You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your presentation and add a voice-over. If you are unable to add voice-over to your PowerPoint,? you will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent)
Option 1:Option 2:You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your presentation and add a voice-over.
- If you are unable to add voice-over to your PowerPoint, you will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your presentation.
- Next, you will use Screencast-o-Matic (or a similar program) to create a video recording of your screen and voice as your present the information.
- Third, you will upload the video presentation to YouTube so your professor can view it. If you choose this option, you will submit your articles as well as the PowerPoint (or equivalent) file and the link to the YouTube presentation to complete this assignment.
Guidelines:
- The presentation must include both audio (your voice explaining the information) and visual (PowerPoint presentation including text and/or images). Videos should not be used within the presentation.
- The presentation should include the following three aspects:
- An overview of your specific topic and its importance and application in current society.
- Theory related to your topic (please refer to Chapters 1 and 2 for theories related to Human Development)
- Discussion of development in at least three stages (e.g., infancy, early childhood, middle childhood, adolescence, emerging adulthood, young adults, middle age, elderly) unless written permission was given by your instruction to instead do a deeper evaluation of a particular topic during one developmental period.
- The presentation must be 15 minutes long (no more than 20).
- The presentation must include information from at least 5 scholarly sources (if used, the course textbook does not count as one of these 5 sources). At least 3 of these should include original research reports typically contain sections with subtitles such as “Abstract”, “Introduction”, “Methods”, “Results”, “Discussion” and “References”.
- APA style citations should be used within the presentation. A reference section (in APA style) should appear at the end of the presentation.
Resources:
- If you do not have PowerPoint, Google Slides is a free alternative.
- You may need a headset with a microphone to record your presentation if your computer does not have a built-in microphone.
- Click here for a brief introduction to using Screencast-o-Matic. The program will create a video file that can be saved on your computer and shared via YouTube.
- PowerPoint 101: Everything You Need to Make a Basic Presentation
- 10 Tips for More Effective PowerPoint Presentations
- 10 Tips for a Good Presentation
- My topic is Bilingual Aphasia . please add pictures and notes to power point. The first article that was chosen in week one is attached.
Title Page
Does the Thematic Hierarchy hold in people with aphasia and across the lifespan? Evidence from the Event Task.
by
Sophia Norvilas
Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the
Dietrich School of Arts and Sciences and
Fredrick Honors College in partial fulfillment
of the requirements for the degree of
Bachelor of Philosophy
University of Pittsburgh
2022
ii
Commit tee Membership Page
UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH
DIETRICH SCHOOL OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
This thesis was presented
by
Sophia Norvilas
It was defended on
November 11, 2022
and approved by
Dr. Tessa Warren, Professor, Psychology
Dr. Julie Fiez, Professor, Psychology
Dr. Jamie Reilly, Professor, Communication Science and Disorders
Thesis Advisor: Dr. Michael Walsh Dickey, Professor, Communication Science and Disorders
iii
Copyright © by Sophia Norvilas
2022
iv
Abstract
Does the Thematic Hierarchy hold in people with aphasia and across the lifespan? Evidence from the Event Task.
Sophia Norvilas
University of Pittsburgh, 2022
Aphasia is a neurological disorder that can disrupt language production and
comprehension, impairing both written and spoken language (Dresang et al., 2019). This condition
is typically brought on by brain damage following a stroke. Research indicates that people with
aphasia sometimes rely on event knowledge to compensate for their language impairment
(Caramazza & Zurif, 1976). However, we know little about event processing in people with
aphasia (PWA), even though event knowledge supports a multitude of crucial cognitive processes,
including language comprehension, language production, memory, and perception. One type of
event knowledge that has been studied thoroughly in linguistics is the entities, objects, and
locations (event roles) that are involved in events. Linguists have developed a hypothesis, known
as the Thematic Hierarchy, that some of these event roles are more cognitively salient than others.
The research I present here uses evidence from a new assessment that measures event knowledge
(the Event Task) to evaluate whether this thematic knowledge is maintained in older adults and
people with aphasia, while also examining whether the performance of PWA on the Event Task is
aligned with the Thematic Hierarchy. PWA (N = 26) and neurologically healthy adults (N = 182)
completed the Event Task, which instructed participants to identify whether a depicted event was
plausible or implausible. Analyses showed that the Thematic Hierarchy did not appear to guide
the performance of PWA or neurologically healthy adults across the lifespan. However, PWA and
neurologically healthy controls alike displayed the same patterns of both accuracy and reaction
v
time performance. In neurologically healthy adults, increased age was correlated with decreased
accuracy and increased reaction times. In addition, neurologically healthy adults with 12+ years
of education were found to have increased accuracy and decreased reaction times. The current
findings could be the foundation for future research regarding aphasia and event knowledge.
vi
Table of Contents
Preface ………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. x
1.0 Introduction …………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 1
1.1 Semantic memory and event knowledge ……………………………………………………………. 1
1.2 The Thematic Hierarchy …………………………………………………………………………………… 2
1.3 The Event Task ………………………………………………………………………………………………… 5
1.4 The Current Study …………………………………………………………………………………………… 7
2.0 Methods …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 8
2.1 Participants ……………………………………………………………………………………………………… 8
2.2 Event Task Stimuli and Procedure ……………………………………………………………………. 8
2.3 Data Compilation ……………………………………………………………………………………………… 9
2.4 Event Taxonomy Questionnaire ………………………………………………………………………. 10
2.5 Data Analysis …………………………………………………………………………………………………. 11
3.0 Results …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 12
3.1 PWA and Matched Controls Analyses …………………………………………………………….. 12
3.2 PWA Analyses ………………………………………………………………………………………………… 14
3.3 Lifespan Analyses …………………………………………………………………………………………… 17
3.3.1 Age Analyses ………………………………………………………………………………………….18
3.3.2 Mini-Mental State Examination Analyses ……………………………………………….20
3.3.3 Years of Education Analyses …………………………………………………………………..22
4.0 Discussion………………………………………………………………………………………………………………. 24
4.1 PWA and Matched Controls …………………………………………………………………………… 25
vii
4.2 Neurologically Healthy Adults Across the Lifespan ………………………………………….. 27
4.3 Clinical Application ………………………………………………………………………………………… 30
4.4 Limitations …………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 34
5.0 Conclusion …………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 35
Bibliography ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 37
viii
List of Tables
Table 1: Canonical and Exclusive Event Role Violations ………………………………………………. 11
ix
List of Figures
Figure 1: PWA and matched control participants' accuracy performance. ……………………. 13
Figure 2: PWA and matched control participants’ reaction time performance. ……………… 14
Figure 3: Individual PWA accuracy performance per event role violation type. …………….. 16
Figure 4: Individual PWA reaction time performance per event role violation type ………. 17
Figure 5: Healthy participants’ accuracy performance in relation to age. ……………………… 18
Figure 6: Healthy participants’ reaction time performance in relation to age. ……………….. 19
Figure 7: Healthy participants’ accuracy performance in relation to MMSE Score. ………. 20
Figure 8: Healthy participants’ reaction time performance in relation to MMSE Score. … 21
Figure 9: Healthy participants’ accuracy performance in relation to years of education. .. 22
Figure 10: Healthy participants’ reaction time performance in relation to years of education.
…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………. 23
x
Preface
First and foremost, I would like to express my gratitude to Dr. Michael Walsh Dickey, my
mentor and Bachelor of Philosophy advisor. Thank you for the years of guidance, encouragement,
and insight. I will never forget the invaluable lessons he taught me and the countless hours he
dedicated to my project. Joining the Language and Brain Lab and learning under his guidance truly
changed the trajectory of my career, and I cannot express the full extent of my appreciation. In
addition, I am so lucky to have studied under Dr. Tessa Warren, my incredible Honors in
Psychology advisor. Her mentorship has been imperative in honing my writing skills and finding
my voice in the scientific community. Her dedication and expertise have been instrumental in
helping me become the researcher I am today.
I am very grateful to Dr. Julie Fiez and Dr. Jamie Reilly, for their enthusiasm and
willingness to serve on my thesis committee. I would like to acknowledge the University of
Pittsburgh Honors College for providing this wonderful opportunity and supporting my work with
resources such as the Brackenridge Fellowship. I am extremely appreciative of Emily Goldberg,
for assisting with data compilation efforts and cheering me on since my very first lab presentation
over two years ago. Finally, I would like to thank my friends and family, for providing unwavering
support and encouragement as I pursued this project.
1
1.0 Introduction
1.1 Semantic memory and event knowledge
Semantic memory is an individual’s long-lasting memory for general knowledge about the
world (Dresang et al., 2019). From birth, we begin collecting knowledge about the world. Semantic
memory is critical when trying to produce or understand language. For example, when you hear a
word or sentence, you consult your knowledge about objects, people, actions, and things to
understand the meaning of that sentence. Semantic memory is often impaired in PWA, and an
accurate measure of semantic memory impairment is crucial for treatment (Antonucci & Reilly,
2008). Existing picture-based assessments of object-related semantic memory include Pyramids
and Palm Trees (PPT) (Howard et al., 1992) and the Camels and Cactus Test (CCT) (Bozeat et al.,
2000). There is also a picture-based assessment of action-related semantic memory called the
Kissing and Dancing Test (KDT) (Bak & Hodges, 2003). However, there are few tests to measure
semantic memory for events in PWA (Dresang et al., 2019).
Events can be defined as collections of people, objects, actions, places, and the context in
which they occur (Dresang et al, 2019). Event roles (or thematic roles) are the people, places, and
things that play a role in an event. The agent is the initiator of an action (Ünal et al., 2021). The
patient is the entity undergoing the effect of some action, often undergoing some change in state
(Ünal et al., 2021). The instrument is the tool used to perform an action (Ünal et al., 2021). The
location is the place in which something is situated (Saeed et. al, 1996). The goal is the entity
towards which something moves, either literally or metaphorically (Ünal et al., 2021). An example
of this would be the following sentence: “The boy hit the baseball with his bat on the field.” The
2
agent would be “the boy” as he is the one doing the act. The action is hit. The patient is the baseball
because it is moved by the action. The instrument is the bat because the boy uses the bat as a tool
to hit the ball. The location is the baseball field on which he is playing the game.
Event role knowledge is activated during language processing. Some evidence for this
comes from a study in which adult participants were given line drawings of two-participant
events—for example, a man throwing a football to a woman—and were asked to describe the
events (Griffin & Bock, 2000). Eye-tracking data showed that the participants’ eye movements
were guided by event features prior to sentence formulation. Participants gazed at the agent prior
to speech, then looked to the patient as they began speaking, and finally returned their gaze to the
agent. The results showed that participants “rapidly extract roles of event participants” while
describing the line drawing (Griffin & Bock, 2000). Event knowledge provides context that helps
individuals understand novel events by providing prototypical events for comparison (McRae &
Matsuki, 2009). Language processing is reliant on event knowledge, as it supports verb retrieval
and thematic-role processing (Dresang et al., 2019).
1.2 The Thematic Hierarchy
The Thematic Hierarchy is the idea that certain event roles are more prominent than others
(Ünal et al., 2021). An event role near the top of the hierarchy would have more prominence,
meaning that it should be identified earlier and processed more quickly than a non-prominent event
role. Agents are at the top of the hierarchy and are most salient or prominent. Next are Patients,
followed by Goals. Instruments are second-to-lowest on the thematic hierarchy. Finally, locations
are the least prominent role. Linguistic research indicates that agents are most prominent because
3
they are linguistically encoded as subjects (Baker, 1997; Jackendoff, 1990). Patients are next
because they are direct objects of the verb. Goals, instruments, and locations are the least likely to
be in these syntactic positions, leading to a lower prominence on the thematic hierarchy.
Instruments are rarely selected as verb arguments and even referred to as “secondary roles” (Baker,
1997). This hierarchy naturally predicts that event roles towards the top of the Thematic Hierarchy
will be mentioned more frequently or identified more easily than event roles from the bottom of
the Thematic Hierarchy.
I will now outline the experimental evidence supporting the Thematic Hierarchy. Agents
and patients are expected to be prioritized over other event roles because of their positions towards
the top of the Thematic Hierarchy. Consistent with this, Hafri et al (2018) found that information
about agents and patients is often extracted even when it is not directly encouraged by a task or
while the participant is preoccupied with another task (Hafri et al., 2018). Participants viewed an
array of stimuli images depicting a simple interaction between a male and a female and were
instructed to answer which character, the male or the female, was on the left or right half of the
screen. When presented with subsequent images in which the event role of the target character
differed from one trial to the next, the participants displayed an event role switch cost, a delay in
reaction time caused by the shift in event role. Hafri et al.’s study found that although task
instructions did not explicitly refer to event roles, participants’ reaction times were affected by the
event role change, indicating that event role information is spontaneously encoded even when not
directly related to the task at hand.
Research suggests that instruments are not prioritized unless the instrument is deemed
particularly relevant to the story being told. Studies show that English speaking adults consistently
omit instruments in retelling stories involving instrument events (Lockridge & Brennan, 2002).
4
The instrument is even more likely to be omitted from the retelling if it is a common or typical
instrument. Lockridge and Brennan (2002) found that during story retellings, atypical instruments
were most often explicitly disclosed to the partner early in the story, whereas an instrument that is
inferable from the story was not mentioned as frequently. These findings suggest that although
instruments can provide crucial information about an event, they are not attended to with the same
focus that an agent or patient would receive, which is consistent with their low role on the Thematic
Hierarchy.
Locations are positioned at the bottom of the Thematic Hierarchy. Ferretti et al (2001) used
single word priming to investigate how verbs activate event role knowledge (Ferretti et al., 2001).
Results showed that verbs primed all event roles except for locations (Ferretti et al., 2001). These
findings are consistent with the idea that locations are the least salient of all event roles, resulting
in location’s place at the bottom of the Thematic Hierarchy.
From a cognitive perspective, the Thematic Hierarchy would predict that entities in more
prominent event roles would be remembered better and identified more easily than entities in less
prominent roles. This claim is supported by a study by Ünal et al. (2021) that tested young learners
of English and Turkish in order to determine whether asymmetries in event role prominence
generalize to other languages. The study had two parts: a linguistic description task and a change-
blindness task. In the linguistic description task, participants were asked to describe event stimuli
images. Event role prioritization was measured by how frequently participants mentioned each
event role when describing a stimuli image. For the change-blindness task, four new stimuli images
were created from each original stimuli image by changing the color of one event role. In the task,
the original stimuli image was displayed, followed by a grey screen. Next, the new stimuli image
featuring the color change was shown, followed by another grey screen. The cycle would then
5
repeat. Participants were instructed to respond as soon as they detected the changing object. In this
task, event role prioritization was measured by how quickly the participant responded to the
changed event role.
Findings suggested that agents were prioritized in both language groups. Patients and goals
tended to be similarly prioritized over instruments in both languages as well. The relative salience
of event roles was similar across young learners of both English and Turkish (Ünal et al., 2021).
This is particularly interesting because even though there is variation in the way these event roles
are encoded across the two languages, event roles were prioritized in similar ways, which aligns
with the Thematic Hierarchy. My thesis explores the theory that people should be faster and more
accurate to detect incongruency when it appears in more prominent roles than in less prominent
roles.
1.3 The Event Task
The Event Task is a picture-based assessment of semantic memory. The preliminary
version of the assessment was developed in the Language and Brain Lab at the University of
Pittsburgh. The task’s goal is to measure how functional or impaired an individual’s ability to
access their semantic memory for events is. In the task, the participant is given photographs of
events that are either plausible or implausible. Plausible events would be those that would
commonly occur in everyday life, such as a person doing work at a desk or boxing in a gym.
Implausible events are impossible or unlikely, and do not match our everyday experience or our
world knowledge based in semantic memory. Images like a man playing the violin underwater and
a woman with her head in a bag are clear examples of implausible events taken directly from the
6
Event stimuli. In each implausible image, at least one event role is incongruent with the event
depicted. Participants were shown one image at a time and were asked to indicate if this scene was
something that may normally happen.
A study conducted by Dresang et al. (2019) assessed the preliminary validity of the Event
Task. The study’s first aim was to characterize typical performance and distinguish stimulus
characteristics of the Event Task. To that end, it gathered data from 90 neurologically healthy
adults across the lifespan and identified both average performance at multiple age-ranges and
variation in processing time across the plausible and implausible images (Dresang et al., 2019).
The second aim was to establish the Event Task’s sensitivity to neurological impairment. Findings
concluded that the Event Task was successful in detecting those participants afflicted with aphasia,
indicated by distinctions between neurologically healthy age-matched controls and PWA (Dresang
et al., 2019). The third and fourth aims were to demonstrate that the Event Task shows preliminary
evidence of convergent validity with measures of language processing and assessments of action
and object knowledge. Poor Event Task performance was correlated with greater deficits in verb
retrieval (specifically, verb production) and thematic-role processing (specifically, producing
agent, patient, and other thematic roles in sentences), and Event Task performance was predicted
by KDT (action based semantic memory assessment) performance and PPT (object based semantic
memory assessment) performance (Dresang et al., 2019). These findings suggest that Event Task
performance is indicative of verb retrieval abilities and thematic-role processing abilities, which
are crucial for language processing. The current study expands upon the foundational work of
Dresang et al. by examining whether the Thematic Hierarchy’s predictions regarding the salience
of various event roles hold for Dresang et al.’s Event Task data.
7
1.4 The Current Study
My research investigated performance on the Event Task in relation to the Thematic
Hierarchy. My research tested whether there was a correlation between the relative prominence of
event roles according to the Thematic Hierarchy (agents > patients > instruments > locations) and
accuracy and reaction time for implausible Event Task pictures which contain violations of our
expectations about those event roles. For example, the stimulus image of a man playing the violin
in the ocean would be a location violation, as the location (the ocean) is not a typical place for that
event to occur. I hypothesized that there would be a positive correlation between Thematic
Hierarchy position and accuracy, with higher accuracy for more prominent event roles. I
hypothesized that there would be a negative correlation between Thematic Hierarchy position and
reaction time, with lower (or faster) reaction times for more prominent event roles.
If the event roles identified as incongruent most accurately and quickly were those event
roles near the top of the Thematic Hierarchy, that would suggest the Thematic Hierarchy is a good
indicator of event role prominence. If the Thematic Hierarchy proves to be a good indicator of
which event roles elicit the fastest or most accurate responses in the Event Task, then it might
suggest that aphasia treatment could be adjusted to focus on improving comprehension of those
event role categories that PWA respond to inaccurately or slowly. If the predicted relationship
between the Thematic Hierarchy and Event Task performance is not found, that would suggest
that the Thematic Hierarchy does not guide event roles in relation to PWA.
8
2.0 Methods
2.1 Participants
As reported in Dresang et al. (2019), Event Task data was collected from 208 participants,
including 26 PWA and 182 neurologically healthy participants across the lifespan. These healthy
participants were divided into groups of 15 in the following age ranges: 20-29, 30-39, 40-49, 50-
59, 60-69, 70-79. The healthy control groups allowed for Event Task performance analysis across
the lifespan. All healthy participants were required to have no history of language, speech, or
neurological impairments. All participants attained a score of >24 on the Mini-Mental State
Examination, an assessment of cognitive function (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975). All participants
were monolingual native English speakers with normal or corrected-to-normal hearing and vision.
All participants provided informed consent and received compensation for their participation in
the study (Dresang et al., 2019). 4 participants were found to have incomplete Event Task data,
resulting in exclusion from the analysis.
2.2 Event Task Stimuli and Procedure
The Event Task is composed of 260 colored images, including 256 experimental images
and 4 practice images (Dresang et al., 2019). The photographs depict people engaging in basic
actions in a variety of environments. Half of these images portray plausible events, such as a person
boxing at a gym, and the other half portray implausible events, such as a man playing the violin
9
underwater. Implausible events are impossible or unlikely, and do not match our everyday
experience or our world knowledge based in semantic memory. Implausible event images were
characterized as stimuli that “violates so-called ‘world knowle
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.