Read the following three research articles and complete written response to the readings. Write a page and a half synthesis
Read the following three research articles and complete written response to the readings. Write a page and a half synthesis of the three articles plus 1 discussion question per article.
The following factors will be considered in grading: relevance, accuracy, synthetization of the reading materials, degree to which the responses show understanding/comprehension of the material, and quality of writing.
· Questions must be original, thoughtful and not easily found in the readings.
· Follows APA Rules
· Use proper citations
· Use past tense when discussing the studies (the research was already conducted).
· Avoid the use of the following words: me, you, I, we, prove, proof
· Refer to the articles by their authors (year of publication) (not by the title of the article or the words first, second, or third)
· Do not just summarize the articles. Dig deeper!
***FOLLOW THE ATTACHED SAMPLE
Two Factor Model of ASD Symptoms
One of the key factors in determining whether an individual has Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is in their social and communication skills. Individuals who are diagnosed with ASD have delayed joint attention, eye gazing, and other social interactions such as pointing (Swain et al., 2014).
Joint attention is an important social skill to master because it is a building block for developing theory of mind which, helps us to understand other’s perspectives. Korhonen et al. (2014) found that individuals with autism have impaired joint attention. However, some did not show impairment in joint attention, which lead to evidence that suggests there are different trajectories for joint attention. One suggestion as to why Korhonen et al. (2014) found mixed results, is that there is evidence that joint attention may not be directly linked to individuals with ASD since they were unable to find a difference in joint attention between ASD and developmentally delayed (DD) individuals. Another suggestion for the mixed results, is individual interest in the task vary. Research has found that while individualized studies are beneficial in detecting personal potential and abilities, it would be difficult to generalize the study in order to further research to ASD as a whole (Korhonen et al., 2014). In addition to joint attention, atypical gaze shifts is a distinguishing factor in individuals with ASD. Swain et al. (2014) found the main difference between typically developing (TD) and ASD individuals in the first 12 months of life is in gaze shifts. Individuals that were diagnosed with ASD earlier had lower scores on positive affect, joint attention, and gaze shifts, however those diagnosed later differed from typically developing (TD) only in gaze shifts. It is not until 24 months that later onset ASD individuals significantly differ from their TD peers, by displaying lower positive affect and gestures (Swain et al., 2014). These findings may lead to other ASD trajectories.
Another defining characteristic of ASD is the excess of restrictive patterns of interest and repetitive motor movements. These patterns and movements often impaired the individual from completing daily tasks. Like joint attention and gaze shifts, these repetitive movements and patterns of interest have different trajectories (Joseph et al., 2013). Joseph et al. (2013) found that individuals with high cognitive functioning ASD engage in more distinct and specific interests and less in repetitive motor movements than individuals with lower cognitive functioning ASD. Another finding showed that at the age of two, repetitive motor and play patterns were more common than compulsion. By the age of four all these behaviors increased however, repetitive use of specific objects was found to be less frequent in older children than younger children. This finding suggests that the ritualistic behaviors and motor movements may present themselves differently based on the age of the individual (Joseph et al., 2013).
Joseph et al. (2013), Korhornen et al. (2014), and Swain et al. (2014) all defined key characteristics of an ASD individual and explains the different trajectories of each characteristic. The difficulty with the trajectories is that it is specific to each individual, some symptoms may worsen while others remain stable. It is also difficult to generalize finding with small sample sizes (Joseph et al., 2013).
Discussion Questions:
1. Korhonen et al. (2014) did not use preference-based stimuli to look for joint attention and did not separate high- from low-functioning ASD individuals. Do you think that there could be a difference in level of motivation from each group? If so, how do you think this could change the results?
2. Swain et al. (2014) found that early and late onset of ASD did not differ in their social skills scores at the age of 12 months. If we know that their social skills do not differ then, is there another factor that would allow diagnosis of late onset ASD to be diagnosed at an earlier point in development?
3. Joseph et al. (2013) explains that it is difficult to assess the trajectories of ASD with a small sample size however, how do you think that their findings still help advance the research on ASD?
,
Abstract Detecting early signs of autism is essential for timely diagnosis and initiation of effective inter-
ventions. Several research groups have initiated pro-
spective studies of high-risk populations including
infant siblings, to systematically collect data on early
signs within a longitudinal design. Despite the potential
advantages of prospective studies of young children at
high-risk for autism, there are also significant meth-
odological, ethical and practical challenges. This paper
outlines several of these challenges, including those
related to sampling (e.g., defining appropriate com-
parison groups), measurement and clinical implications
(e.g., addressing the needs of infants suspected of
having early signs). We suggest possible design and
implementation strategies to address these various
challenges, based on current research efforts in
the field and previous studies involving high-risk
populations.
Keywords Early identification Æ Screening Æ Longitudinal studies Æ Prospective studies Æ Infant Æ Autism Æ Child development Æ Siblings
Please note that the opinion and assertions contained herein are the private opinions of the authors and are not to be con- strued as official or as representing the views of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, the National Institute of Mental Health, or the National Institutes of Health.
L. Zwaigenbaum (&) Department of Paediatrics, McMaster Children’s Hospital at McMaster University, PO Box 2000, Hamilton, Ontario L8N 3Z5, Canada e-mail: [email protected]
A. Thurm Division of Pediatric Translational Research and Treatment Development, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
W. Stone Departments of Pediatrics and Psychology & Human Development, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
G. Baranek Division of Occupational Science, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
S. Bryson Departments of Pediatrics and Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, USA
J. Iverson Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
A. Kau Center for Developmental Biology and Perinatal Medicine, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Bethesda, MD, USA
A. Klin Yale Child Study Centre, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
C. Lord Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
R. Landa Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD, USA
J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:466–480
DOI 10.1007/s10803-006-0179-x
123
O R I G I N A L P A P E R
Studying the Emergence of Autism Spectrum Disorders in High-risk Infants: Methodological and Practical Issues
Lonnie Zwaigenbaum Æ Audrey Thurm Æ Wendy Stone Æ Grace Baranek Æ Susan Bryson Æ Jana Iverson Æ Alice Kau Æ Ami Klin Æ Cathy Lord Æ Rebecca Landa Æ Sally Rogers Æ Marian Sigman
Published online: 4 August 2006 � Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006
Introduction
Overview
Several reviews over the past decade have highlighted
the importance of early recognition and specialized
intervention for improving outcomes for children with
autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Dawson & Oster-
ling, 1997; Rogers, 1996; Smith, Groen, & Wynn, 2000).
Although recent service registry (Croen, Grether,
Hoogstrate, & Selvin, 2002) and population-based data
(Yeargin-Alsopp, et al., 2003) suggest that more chil-
dren are being diagnosed prior to age 4 years than in
the past, a formal diagnosis may still lag years behind
the time when parents initially identify concerns
(Coonrod & Stone, 2004; Howlin & Moore, 1997;
Siegel, Pliner, Eschler, & Elliott, 1988). As a result,
interest has increased in identifying and raising
awareness regarding the characteristics of ASD present
at young ages (Bryson, Zwaigenbaum & Roberts, 2004;
Landa, 2003). In addition to improving outcomes,
earlier diagnosis allows parents the opportunity to
receive counseling regarding current estimates of
recurrence risk in autism, which they may take into
account in future family planning. Research to date
supports the conclusions that one can: (1) reliably
diagnose as young as 24 months (Lord, 1995; Stone
et al., 1999); and (2) observe the behavioral markers of
autism well before 24 months (e.g., Dahlgren & Gill-
berg, 1989; Ohta, Nagai, Hara, & Sasaki, 1987; Rogers
& DiLalla, 1990).
Most of the work aimed at identifying early signs of
ASD has been retrospective, focusing on early behav-
ioral evidence of the disorder in children who have
already received a diagnosis. The most common
methods used to gather information about earlier
behaviors have been retrospective reports from parents
and analysis of early home videotapes. Although
research using these approaches has supported clinical
efforts aimed at earlier detection, many questions
regarding early signs, their timing, and their underlying
developmental mechanisms remain. Prospective
research into the early development of ASD in
high-risk infants is an exciting new frontier, and can
potentially answering these questions more systemati-
cally, while avoiding some of the biases associated with
retrospective designs. In this paper, we outline the
theoretical advantages and general feasibility of pro-
spective studies of young children at high-risk for
ASD, and acknowledge and discuss the significant
methodological, ethical and practical challenges that
accompany these studies. Issues discussed include the
design of high-risk studies, selection of comparison
groups, measurement of developmental delay and
deviance, generalizability, and clinical interpretation of
findings.
Identifying Early Signs of Autism using
Retrospective Designs
Retrospective parental reports offer a unique window
into early behaviors of children with ASD, as parents
have the advantage of observing their children’s
behavior over time and across a variety of settings.
Investigators report a wide range of symptoms that are
more common in children with autism under the age of
24 months than similar-aged children with develop-
mental delays or mental retardation (DD). Early
symptoms associated with autism cross several devel-
opmental domains, including social behavior (Dahlgren
& Gillberg, 1989; De Giacomo & Fombonne, 1998;
Hoshino et al., 1982; Ohta et al., 1987; Young, Brewer,
& Pattison, 2003), communication (Dahlgren &
Gillberg, 1989; De Giacomo & Fombonne, 1998; Ohta
et al., 1987; Young et al., 2003), affective expression
(Dahlgren & Gillberg, 1989; De Giacomo &
Fombonne, 1998; Hoshino et al., 1982), and sensory
hypo- and hypersensitivities (Dahlgren & Gillberg,
1989; De Giacomo & Fombonne, 1998; Hoshino et al.,
1982). These findings have been very important in
guiding further research aimed at identifying early signs
of ASD. However, a number of factors limit parents’
ability to provide accurate descriptions of early
behaviors. First, a parent’s incidental observations
regarding the subtle social and communicative differ-
ences that characterize young children with autism may
be limited compared to systematic assessment by
trained clinicians (Stone, Hoffman, Lewis, & Ousley,
1994). Moreover, their tendency to use compensatory
strategies to elicit their child’s best behaviors (with or
without their awareness) may affect their behavioral
descriptions (Baranek, 1999). Retrospective parental
R. Landa Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
S. Rogers Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California at Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA
M. Sigman Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:466–480 467
123
reports may be also be prone to errors and distortions of
recall, especially when one asks parents to remember
behaviors that occurred many years ago. In particular,
having already received a diagnosis of autism for their
child, parents may bias their reports toward behaviors
that are consistent with the diagnosis. A recent retro-
spective study overcame some of these problems by
gathering information about behaviors under
24 months from parents of preschoolers before they had
received a diagnosis (Wimpory, Hobson, Williams, &
Nash, 2000). However, limitations of this methodology
remain, as retrospective reports are not generally
informative on the issue of whether differences in early
social and communicative development are best char-
acterized by delayed emergence, reduced frequency, or
truly abnormal or deviant quality of fundamental skills
such as joint attention.
A second strategy for obtaining retrospective
information about characteristics of autism present
before 24 months is the analysis of early videotapes of
children made by their parents. This approach has
significant strengths relative to retrospective parental
reports: it allows the observation of behaviors as they
occur in familiar and natural settings, and enables
objective rating of behavior by unbiased observers.
However, this methodology is not without its limita-
tions, the foremost being that parents record these
tapes to preserve family memories, rather than to
document their child’s behavior across a variety of
settings. As a result, tapes from different families will
naturally vary as a function of the quality of the
recording, the activities and settings that were
recorded, and the length of time the child is visible.
Moreover, if children do not behave as expected (or
desired), parents may re-record taped segments until
they obtain the desired response. Efforts to standardize
tapes across families can be extremely difficult and
time intensive (Baranek, 1999). Moreover, most
studies employing home videotapes have used children
with typical development (TD) rather than those with
DD as comparison groups, which limits the extent of
our knowledge about autism-specific deficits. Behav-
iors found to differentiate children with ASD from
children with TD under 24 months by at least two
studies are: responding to name (Baranek, 1999;
Osterling & Dawson, 1994; Osterling, Dawson, &
Munson, 2002), looking at others (Adrien et al., 1993;
Maestro et al., 2002; Osterling & Dawson, 1994;
Osterling et al., 2002), smiling at others (Adrien et al.,
1993; Maestro et al., 2002), and motor stereotypies
(Adrien et al., 1993; Baranek, 1999). Only two studies
published to date compared behaviors of children with
ASD with those of children with DD younger than
24 months; these found that children with ASD were
less likely to respond to their name (Baranek, 1999;
Osterling et al., 2002) and to look at others (Osterling
et al., 2002). Notably, analysis of home videos also
highlights that there is may be a subgroup of ‘‘late
onset’’ children whose early behavioral symptoms are
less apparent (Osterling et al., 2002; Werner, Dawson,
Osterling, & Dinno, 2000).
Potential Advantages of Prospective Studies
Retrospective parental reports and analyses of home
videos can help guide the development of early iden-
tification and screening procedures (as argued by Fil-
ipek et al., 1999), but these procedures must ultimately
be validated empirically in prospective studies, with
sufficient follow-up of both screen positive and screen
negative children to allow estimates of sensitivity and
specificity. In fact, prospective studies of high-risk
infants (which, until recently, have been rare in autism)
may also identify novel behavioral (and biological)
markers that show the way forward in developing more
effective early identification and screening measures.
Prospective studies are not subject to recall biases, they
can be designed to examine specific constructs of
interest, and they can provide comparable data col-
lection points and methods across children. Perhaps,
most importantly, these studies allow collection of data
longitudinally across different ages, which can foster
our understanding of developmental trajectories and
the impact of early delays in one domain (e.g., social
orienting) on the subsequent development of another
(e.g., language).
High-risk samples have informed studies of other
neurodevelopmental and neuropsychiatric conditions,
including language/reading disorders (Carroll &
Snowling, 2004), attention deficit hyperactivity disor-
der (Faraone, Biederman, Mennin, Gershon, & Tsu-
ang, 1996), bipolar affective disorder (Chang, Steiner,
& Ketter, 2000; Geller, Tillman, Craney, & Bolhofner,
2004), and schizophrenia (Schubert & McNeil, 2004).
Prospective studies of siblings and offspring of affected
probands have generated significant insights regarding
premorbid development and predictors of illness in
these high-risk groups. For example, children with
schizophrenic parents have attention and verbal
memory deficits, gross motor delays, and dysfunction
of smooth-pursuit eye movements (Erlenmeyer-Kim-
ling, 2000; Schubert & McNeil, 2004), and children with
a parent or sibling with dyslexia have greater difficulty
with phonological processing than age-matched low-
risk controls, despite normal early language develop-
ment (Carroll & Snowling, 2004). Notably, these
468 J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:466–480
123
studies generally focus on group differences between
high- and low-risk children rather than the association
between early markers and outcome status, because of
insufficient power and/or follow-up. In contrast,
autism can be diagnosed in early childhood, so out-
comes can be determined after a relatively short
follow-up period. Hence, one can study autism pro-
spectively much more easily (i.e., with fewer resources
and with less risk of sample loss) than an adult-onset
disorder such as schizophrenia.
Prospective Studies in Autism: Siblings and other
High-Risk Groups
Several populations at increased risk of ASD that can
be identified in early childhood: children with early
signs of autism or developmental delays (DD) identi-
fied through population screening, children at
increased risk of autism due to specific medical diag-
noses or genetic anomalies, and the main focus of this
paper, infants with an older sibling with ASD.
At least two research groups have studied early
signs of autism in high-risk samples identified by
population screening. Charman et al., (1997) and
Swettenham et al., (1998), reported on a high-risk group
of children who failed the Checklist for Autism in
Toddlers (CHAT; Baron-Cohen, Allen, & Gillberg,
1992), a screening measure administered at 18 months
of age. Detailed assessment of social, communication
and play skills was completed at 20 months, and
diagnostic outcomes were assessed at age of three and a
half. Children subsequently diagnosed with autism were
compared to those subsequently diagnosed with devel-
opmental delay based to their 20-month skills. At this
early age, the children with autism spent less time
looking at adults during free play (Swettenham et al.,
1998), were less likely to look at the face of an adult
feigning distress (Charman et al., 1997), showed less
gaze switching between people and objects (Charman
et al., 1997; Swettenham et al., 1998), and showed less
imitation (Charman et al., 1997). Wetherby et al., (2004)
followed a group of children who had failed communi-
cation screening using the Communication and Sym-
bolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile (CSBS
DP, Wetherby & Prizant, 2002). They obtained video-
tapes of the CSBS Behavior Sample at a mean age of
18–21 months for children who received later diagnoses
of autism, DD, or who were typically developing.
Specific features that differentiated children with
autism from the other two groups include social-
communication behaviors (e.g., reduced eye gaze,
coordination of gaze with other nonverbal behaviors,
directing attention, responding to name, and unusual
prosody) and repetitive body and object use. Notably,
the content of the initial screen (i.e., children are se-
lected based on a particular profile of early signs), may
introduce sampling biases, and the fact that data are
only collected from the point of first screening onward
limits the age range over which autism can be studied.
There are also children at increased risk for autism
due to medical risk factors, such as Fragile X syndrome
(Rogers, Wehner & Hagerman et al., 2001), specific
chromosome abnormalities (Xu, Zwaigenbaum, Szat-
mari & Scherer, 2004), tuberous sclerosis (Bolton &
Griffiths, 1997), and prenatal exposure to valproic acid
(Williams et al., 2001) or thalidomide (Stromland et al.,
2002). However, these specific risk factors are all rel-
atively rare and would be difficult to study in large
numbers, and may be associated with unique clinical
features that may not generalize to other children with
autism.
There is growing interest in studying infant siblings
of children with ASD, who are arguably the most
clearly defined high-risk group available. Notably,
Baron-Cohen et al., (2002) originally developed the
CHAT screening algorithm based on items that, at
18 months, were atypical in four siblings subsequently
diagnosed with autism. More recent reports by Pilow-
sky and colleagues (Pilowski, Yirmiya, Shalev, &
Gross-Tsur, 2003; Pilowski, Yirmiya, Doppelt, Gross-
Tsur, & Shalev, 2004) support the feasibility of study-
ing early development in siblings and Zwaigenbaum
et al., (2005) reported several behavioral markers
which, at 12 months, predict a subsequent diagnosis of
autism in a sibling sample. In addition, Landa & Gar-
rett-Mayer (2006) report developmental levels and
trajectories in that differentiate infant siblings later
diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder, beginning at
6 months of age. Autism is associated with the highest
relative risk in siblings, compared to general popula-
tion of all the neuropsychiatric disorders (Szatmari,
Jones, Zwaigenbaum & MacLean, 1998). Previous
studies found rates of autism in siblings of children
with autism range from 3% to 5%, which is at least
20 times higher than rates of autism in the general
population (Bailey, Phillips & Rutter, 1996; Simonoff,
1998; Szatmari et al., 1998). In fact, estimates of
recurrence risk (that is, the risk to later-born children)
may be as high as 8.6% when one child in the family
has autism, and 35% when two siblings have autism
(Ritvo et al., 1989). Notably, these risk estimates may
be somewhat conservative, as they come mainly from
studies conducted over 20 years ago, using more
restrictive diagnostic criteria (DSM-III).
The risk to relatives of individuals with autism also
extends beyond the traditional boundaries of the
J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:466–480 469
123
autistic spectrum (Bailey, Palferman, Heavey, & Le
Couteur, 1998). Family members have higher rates of
certain psychiatric and developmental disorders, com-
pared to individuals with no family history of autism
(Landa, Folstein & Isaacs, 1991; Landa et al., 1992;
Pickles et al., 2000; Piven, Palmer, Jacobi, Childress, &
Arndt, 1997; Smalley, McCracken, & Tanguay, 1995;
Yirmiya & Shaked, 2005). As well, because of the early
age of diagnosis of the proband, one can ascertain sib-
lings in early infancy or even prenatally, making it
possible to study early neurodevelopmental mecha-
nisms, and to partially avoid (or at least to systemati-
cally measure) the impact of potentially confounding
environmental factors. Infant sibling research offers
unique opportunities to study the neural origins and
developmental cascade that leads to autism, potentially
providing new insights into its neurobiology, improved
methods of early detection, and earlier opportunities for
intervention.
In August 2003, the National Alliance for Autism
Research (NAAR) and the National Institute of Child
Health and Human Development (NICHD) co-spon-
sored a workshop for researchers engaged in the study
of populations of young children at high-risk for autism,
particularly siblings of children with autism. Despite the
theoretical advantages and exciting opportunities
associated with this research design, there are clearly
significant methodological, ethical and practical chal-
lenges facing researchers studying young children at
high-risk for autism. In the remainder of this paper we
outline several of these challenges, including those
related to sampling (e.g., recruitment of adequately
sized samples, determining inclusion/exclusion criteria
for high-risk infants and appropriate comparison
groups), measurement (e.g., selection of constructs and
measures) and clinical implications (e.g., clinical man-
agement of infants who appear to have early signs of
ASD). We suggest possible design and implementation
solutions for these various challenges, based on current
research efforts in the field and previous studies
involving high-risk populations. These issues have
implications not only for research with infant siblings,
but also for research in other aspects of early charac-
terization and diagnosis of autism.
Issues Related to Sampling
Sample Size
High-risk studies in other fields (e.g., schizophrenia,
dyslexia) have generally been designed to compare
siblings with controls on a group basis, without
knowing the ultimate outcomes of individual siblings
(Carroll & Snowling, 2004; Erlenmeyer-Kimling,
2000). Initial infant sibling studies of ASD by Pilowski
and colleagues (2003, 2004) also focus on group com-
parisons. However, if the main objective of a sibling
study is to identify early markers that are predictive of
a specific diagnosis, then individual outcomes become
important, and one must power the sample size with
reference to the expected number of participants who
will have the diagnosis of interest (i.e., not the total
number of infants enrolled).
The required sample size for these studies will
depend on the specific research question posed. A few
issues are considered for illustration. First, if one
defines the outcome of interest more broadly (e.g.,
language delay), there will be a larger number of sib-
lings with that outcome, potentially making it easier to
detect differences between ‘affected’ and ‘unaffected’
siblings. However, predictors of secondary outcomes
such as language delay may not generalize outside of an
autism sibling sample, limiting the clinical utility of such
findings. A second issue to consider is the strength of
the association between the predictor variables and
outcome under study (e.g., the sensitivity and specificity
of early markers for the subsequent diagnosis of aut-
ism), which will influence the power to detect a rela-
tionship. However, the investigator may sometimes
select predictor variables on a theoretical basis, so the
actual strength of the relation between predictor and
outcome variables may be difficult to estimate with
confidence. A third variable to consider is the number
of outcomes/variables being studied; for example,
contrasting siblings by more than two outcomes (e.g.,
ASD versus developmental delay versus typical devel-
opment), or examining the effects of stratification
variables (e.g., gender) within and across groups may be
of interest, but will require even larger samples sizes.
Due to limitations on numbers of infants born to
older siblings with autism within specified geographic
regions, studies may maximize their efforts to collect
data in a timely fashion by establishing collaborations
across multiple sites and utilizing a common set of
core assessment measures. Such collaborations accel-
erate the process of identifying early predictors of
outcome by increasing the collective sample size so
that investigators can address more refined questions
about outcomes and predictors. Although collabora-
tions between research groups require additional ef-
fort and resources to support the necessary steps of
ensuring consistency in methods and measures, as well
as inter-rater reliability for observations, these proce-
dures allow the examination of consistency of findings
across sites, ensure the fidelity of assessment
470 J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:466–480
123
measures, and facilitate future attempts to repl
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.