For the week’s topics of Conduct Problems and ADHD, analyze the primary arguments presented in either one of additional articles posted on Canvas OR? a rel
For the week's topics of Conduct Problems and ADHD, analyze the primary arguments presented in either one of additional articles posted on Canvas OR a relevant empirical, peer-reviewed article of your choosing.
Discuss how the author's perspective contributes to the broader academic conversation on these subjects. Reflect on the strengths and limitations of the author's arguments, providing specific examples from the text. Include your critical evaluation of the evidence presented and how it supports or contradicts other sources you have encountered or your current knowledge of the study of abnormal child psychology. Ensure you properly cite (APA formatting, 7th edition) the additional articles from Canvas in your discussion.
Feel free to let me know if you need any more assistance.
Vol.:(0123456789)1 3
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-020-00713-9
Inhibitory Control Deficits in Children with Oppositional Defiant Disorder and Conduct Disorder Compared to Attention Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta‑analysis
Mikaela D. Bonham1 · Dianne C. Shanley1 · Allison M. Waters1 · Olivia M. Elvin1
Accepted: 24 September 2020 © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020
Abstract Inhibitory control decits are known to be characteristic of Oppositional Deant Disorder (ODD), Conduct Disorder (CD), and Attention-Decit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD); but it is unclear whether children with ODD/CD have inhibitory control problems independent of ADHD comorbidity. Previous reviews of inhibitory control and ODD/CD have only focused on one type of measure of inhibitory control or used non-clinical samples. The current meta-analysis explored inhibitory control problems of children with ODD/CD by systematically reviewing studies where children have a diagnosis of ODD and/or CD. Comparisons were made across 25 studies between children with ODD/CD, ODD/CD + ADHD, ADHD, and healthy controls (HC) on various measures of inhibitory control and ADHD symptomatology to explore impacts of ADHD comorbidity. A small signicant eect (g = -0.58, p < .001) suggested children with ODD/CD are likely to have more diculties with inhibitory control than healthy children. However, comparisons between clinical groups suggested this eect may be due to ADHD symptomatology present in each group. As diculties with inhibitory control are similar, across clinical groups, a dimensional approach to understanding ODD/CD and ADHD may be more useful to consider in future diagnostic criteria. Similarities across clinical groups highlight that therapeutic approaches that assist children with disruptive behaviours could benet from teaching children and their families how to cope with inhibitory control decits.
Keywords Inhibitory control · Conduct disorder · Oppositional deant disorder · Executive function · Disruptive behaviour
Introduction
Children with disruptive behaviour disorders have diculty regulating emotions and inhibiting undesirable behaviours. Executive function plays an important role in the regulation of thoughts, emotions, and behaviours (Diamond 2013). Recently, empirical studies have highlighted the unique role that executive function decits can play in the aetiology of disruptive behaviours (Ezpeleta and Granero 2014; Hobson et al. 2011). Historically, these neurobiological factors have often been overlooked in favour of psychological and social factors that cause disruptive behaviour. According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-5), disruptive
behaviour disorders are classied as Disruptive, Impulse- Control, and Conduct Disorders (DICCD); including conduct disorder (CD), oppositional deant disorder (ODD), kleptomania, intermittent explosive disorder, pyromania, and other or unspecied disruptive, impulse-control and conduct disorders (American Psychological Association 2013). In the past, Attention Decit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) was classied as a disruptive behaviour disorder, and as such, much of the research on the relationship between executive function deficits and disruptive behaviours has focussed on children with ADHD. However, with the introduction of the DSM-5, ADHD has been reclassified. It is now a Neurodevelopmental Disorder due to empirical evidence that neurobiological deficits (e.g., executive dysfunction) are core characteristics of ADHD (Oosterlaan et al. 1998; Thorell and Wahlstedt 2006; Senderecka et al. 2012).
Interestingly, similar deficits are present in early childhood for children with ODD/CD (Schoemaker et al. 2012), but these disorders have remained in the DICCD chapter. This systematic review uses a meta-analytic
* Mikaela D. Bonham mikaela.bonham@grithuni.edu.au
1 School of Applied Psychology, Menzies Health Institute of Queensland, Grith University, Mt Gravatt, Quensland 4122, Australia
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology
1 3
approach to examine the role of inhibition control, one of the three key components of executive function, in ODD/ CD. It compares the following groups on measures of inhibitory control as well as ADHD symptomatology: ODD/ CD, ODD/CD + ADHD, ADHD, and healthy controls (HC). Systematically reviewing the available empirical evidence will help us to consider whether ODD and CD are better captured within diagnostic manuals as a neurodevelopmental disorder.
Aetiology of ODD and CD
Psychopathology across the l i fespan typical ly develops from the interaction between individual and environmental factors over time (Matthys and Lochman 2017). When considering the aetiology and treatment of disruptive behaviours, learned behaviour and parenting style have received much attention. Children are exposed to disruptive models of behaviour and learn this behaviour from their environment, which in turn develops into a disruptive behavioural disorder over time (Tremblay 2010). This has been demonstrated in children who model behaviour after coercive parents or associate with delinquent peers when there is an absence of positive parenting (Matthys et al. 2012). When disruptive behaviour disorders are explained by coercive parenting or peer inf luences, interventions naturally follow a “learning-based” approach (Matthys et al., 2012, p. 235). Interventions focussed on parenting and behaviour modification for antisocial youths have demonstrated small to moderate effect sizes; with a mean effect size of 0.47 (range -0.06 to 1.68) and 0.35 (range -1.04 to 1.87), respectively (McCart et al. 2006). Similarly, parenting group interventions for externalising behaviours have also demonstrated small to moderate effect sizes; with a mean effect size of -0.38 favouring intervention (range -0.56 to -0.19; Buchanan-Pascall, Gray, Gordon & Melvin, 2018). Matthy and Lochman (2017) argue that although there is demonstrated effectiveness for behavioural parent training, the effect sizes remain small to moderate, which may be due to many studies being conducted in highly controlled environments which may not be representative of real-world practice. Further, there may also be an impact on children’s ability to learn and problem solve due to neurocognitve impairments in areas such as executive function (Matthys and Lochman, 2017; Matthys et al., 2012). Matthys and colleagues (2012) highlighted that the neurocognitive basis of skill deficits in children with ODD and CD is understudied and understanding its role in the development and maintenance of these disorders has important implications for intervention, with investigation into the role of executive function in ODD/CD as being an important next step. Understanding
children’s neurocognitive challenges would be useful to inform more individualised treatment for children and their families (Matthys and Lochman, 2017). This review will be the first to meta-analyse inhibitory control deficits across childhood, in a clinical sample of children with ODD/CD relative to healthy and clinical controls.
Inhibitory Control. Executive function deficits are a key characteristic of ADHD. Inhibitory control is one of the three established domains of executive function (Miyake et al. 2000). Broadly, inhibitory control refers to the ability to withhold an emotional or behavioural response in order to achieve a goal (Best and Miller 2010; Nigg 2000; van Goozen et al. 2004). When there is a skill deficit in this area, children have more difficulty stopping unwanted behaviour. While this is the definition used in the present review, across the literature, there are several ways of conceptualising inhibitory control; definitions tend to differ based on the function of inhibitory control. For example, some have conceptualised inhibitory control as executive, motivational, and attentional inhibitory control (Nigg 2000) or prepotent response inhibition, resistance to distractor interference, and resistance to proactive interference (Friedman and Miyake 2004).
The current review will examine cool and hot inhibitory control separately because inhibitory control may operate differently based on the emotional salience of the task at hand (Zelazo and Carlson 2012; Zelazo et al. 2010). Therefore, inhibitory control may operate as a ‘hot’ function when a person is in affective contexts, where cues for reward or punishment are present; for example, a delayed snack task employing delayed gratification (Zelazo et al. 2010). On the other hand, ‘cool’ inhibitory control is utilised when presented with abstract problems (Zelazo et al. 2010); for example, a go/ no-go behavioural task, assessing a person’s ability to not respond to a stimulus. Fundamentally, hot and cool executive function require different cognitive processes to be executed (Zelazo and Carlson 2012), which suggests that tasks assessing hot and cool inhibitory control should be analysed separately.
Measuring Inhibitory Control. Performance measures and rating scales will be examined separately throughout the review. Accurate measurement of executive function, including inhibitory control is difficult due to the overlapping nature of brain functions. It is impossible to obtain a pure measure of one cognitive process, such as inhibitory control, as all behaviours require more than one cognitive process (Anderson 2002). This issue is known as task impurity, where an outcome from a task or measurement does not solely reect a single ability, because completing the task requires more than one brain function to be executed (Miyake and Friedman 2012). For example,
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology
1 3
the Stroop task is a measure to assess inhibitory control however the task requires reading and comprehension to be completed. If an individual is impaired in either of these areas, it may aect their performance on the task overall. Performance measures of inhibitory control are standardised or experimental tasks that capture a child’s ability to inhibit a response when completing a task. Results of performance measures of executive function are often confounded by noise, as other executive and non-executive functions are contributing to performance (Miyake et al. 2000; Miyake and Friedman 2012). Ratng scales oer an ecological way to assess inhibitory control by documenting informant- or self- report of inhibitory control behaviours. They too continue to be aected by task impurity. Further complicating the measurement of inhibitory control, performance tasks and rating scales have been demonstrated by some researchers to not reect the same construct (Bodnar et al. 2007; Toplak et al. 2013).
Meta-analysis may allow for a way to assess overall inhibitory control performance at a group level, through a pooled eect size across all measures, providing a more global picture of inhibitory control in children with ODD/ CD. Analysing each task within inhibitory control separately makes it dicult to ascertain an overall eect of inhibitory control. However, understanding differences between performance on each task of inhibitory control is important. Prior meta-analyses of children with ADHD and ODD/CD have only reviewed the Stop Task (Lipszyc and Schachar 2010; Oosterlaan et al. 1998). As such, the analyses in the current review will pool all measures (i.e., according to cool vs. hot, and performance measures vs. rating scales) to understand inhibitory control in children with ODD/CD more globally, as well as subgroup analyses by task.
The Relationship Between ODD/CD, ADHD, and Inhibitory Control
This review will explore the similarities and dierences between children with ODD/CD, ADHD, ODD/ CD + ADHD, and healthy controls on measures of inhibitory control and ADHD symptomatology. When compared to their typically developing peers, pre-schoolers with ADHD, hard to manage behaviours, and aggressive behaviours have been found to have more diculties with inhibitory control (Schoemaker et al. 2013). However, there were too few studies to conduct ADHD and disruptive behaviour analyses separately. Oosterlaan and colleagues (1998) identied that children with CD (and no ADHD) had more diculties with inhibitory control compared to typically developing children; however, the review was limited to one measure of inhibitory control (i.e., stop signal task). Similarly, Lipszyc and Schachar (2010) meta-analysed performance on the stop signal task, revealing children with ODD/CD, ADHD,
and ADHD + ODD/CD had worse performance on the stop signal task compared to healthy controls. Greatest eects were found in children with ADHD, followed by ODD/CD and ADHD + ODD/CD respectively. Similarly, others have identied inhibitory control decits for children with ADHD compared to healthy controls (e.g., Wright, Lipszyc, Dupuis, Thayapararajah, Sathees, and Schachar 2014). There is an absence of reviews where performance on measures of hot inhibitory control has been assessed or reported for children with ODD/CD.
Often, behavioural/response inhibition (i.e., cool inhibitory control) is investigated in relation to externalising behaviours such as ODD/CD. Poor response inhibition is thought to be one factor that contributes to externalising behaviour diculties; including conduct problems (Miyake and Friedman 2012). As a result, children with diculties in response inhibition would be expected to have more diculty stopping unwanted behaviours (Hwang et al. 2016). Others have identied more diculties with hot inhibitory control, due to dysfunctional brain circuitry (cortico-striato- thalamo-corticial neurocircuitry) responsible for emotional executive function (Zhu et al. 2018). A review of imaging studies indicated abnormalities in brain regions associated with hot executive functions were more common in children with ODD/CD compared to those with ADHD (Rubia 2011). Further, diculties with emotional responding have been observed in children aged 10 to 18 years, based on fMRI data on an aective stop signal task (Hwang et al. 2016). Despite the inhibitory control decit hypothesis for children with ODD/CD, with a dearth of meta-analytic reviews, the literature lacks consensus as to whether these decits are characteristic of children with ODD/CD. Taken together, our current understanding of inhibitory control deficits in children with ODD/CD is limited; and this is further complicated by the relationship between ODD/CD and ADHD.
ADHD has often been found to be comorbid in girls with CD (OR > 40) and ODD (OR = 79), and boys with CD (OR = 3.7) and ODD (OR = 8.7; Costello et al. 2003). Other studies have also identied high comorbidity between CD and ADHD comorbidity in a community sample of children (OR = 10.7Angold et al. 1999). As ODD/CD and ADHD are often comorbid, it is dicult to ascertain whether decits in inhibitory control are due to the severity of disruptive behaviour in ODD/CD, or whether they may simply be due to sub-clinical levels of ADHD symptoms (Blair et al. 2018). For example, youths aged 10 to 18 years with conduct disorder were found to have more diculties with hot inhibitory control, however this was attributed to the presence of ADHD symptoms, rather than the severity of conduct problems (Hwang et al. 2016). Understanding whether inhibitory control decits are indeed characteristic of children with ODD/CD or if they are simply a result
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology
1 3
of ADHD symptomatology may help us understand if ODD/CD and ADHD are categorically dierent or similar psychopathology within the same dimension.
Aims
The aim of this review was to comprehensively assess whether children and adolescents (3–17 years) with a clinical diagnosis of ODD and/or CD demonstrate inhibitory control decits more than healthy peers, independent of ADHD comorbidity. ODD/CD and ADHD are signicantly comorbid, hence there is a need to examine whether inhibitory control is similar or dierent for these diagnostic categories. Therefore, this paper aims to determine whether children with ODD/CD have greater or lesser inhibitory control decits when compared to children with ADHD or healthy controls (HC). We sought to answer these questions by making comparisons between ODD/CD, ADHD, ODD/ CD + ADHD, and HC groups on measures of inhibitory control and ADHD symptomatology. Additionally, we explored whether there were differences between the aforementioned groups on measures of cool and hot inhibitory control, rating scales, and the implications of measuring inhibitory control. All measures of inhibitory control and ADHD symptomatology were included in the review. Determining if inhibitory control decits are characteristic of children with disruptive disorders will contribute to a more comprehensive aetiological framework, which in turn will inform more focussed intervention strategies.
Methods
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
Studies were included if: (1) they were written in English, (2) sample participants were 3-17 years old, (3) sample participants had a clinical diagnosis of ODD, CD, and/or comorbid ADHD based on ICD-10, DSM-IV, DSM-IV-TR, or DSM-5 criteria using either clinical interviewing or diagnostic measures, (4) outcome measures specically tested for inhibitory control using a performance or rating scale, and (5) they had a healthy control (HC) or ADHD group as a comparison. Studies were excluded if sample participants had intellectual impairment, Autism Spectrum Disorder, or other cognitive impairments as comorbid disorders.
Search Strategy and Study Selection
The meta-analysis followed the recommendations and standards set by the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic
Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA; Moher et al. 2009). A review protocol was registered with PROSPERO prior to completion of title and abstract screening. The initial protocol was amended, and all changes were reected on the published PROSPERO protocol (CRD42019121527). To obtain relevant literature, the following electronic databases were accessed in February 2020: PsycINFO, PubMed, Embase, CINAHL and Scopus. The nal title and abstract searches were conducted using the strings:
(executive function OR cognitive function OR executive dysfunction OR dysexecutive syndrome OR cognitive dysfunction OR executive control OR executive impairment OR inhibition OR inhibitory control OR attentional control OR emotional control OR cognitive control OR effortful control) AND (externalising behaviour OR externalizing behavior OR oppositional defiant disorder OR disruptive behaviour OR disruptive behavior OR problem behaviour OR problem behaviour OR conduct problems OR conduct disorder OR ADHD OR AD/HD OR attention decit hyperactivity disorder OR hyperkinetic) AND (child OR children OR adolescent OR kid OR school OR preschool OR pre-school OR pediatric OR paediatric OR teen OR teenager OR youth OR boy OR girl).
Key words and MESH terms were determined to be ineective in this search, as many records that were found explored executive function as a secondary construct of interest. Therefore, only title and abstract searches were used. Searches did not employ a restriction of year of publication. Reference list checks of review articles (Lipszyc and Schachar 2010; Oosterlaan et al. 1998; Schoemaker et al. 2013) and articles included in the current review were hand-checked to ensure all possible eligible studies were included.
Data Extraction
A total of 10,622 articles were retrieved through database and reference list searches. Following deduplication and assessment for eligibility, a total of 25 studies remained for nal review. Screening and appraisal were completed by two independent reviewers (MB and OE). Data extraction was completed by MB and with cross checks completed by another reviewer (OE). The following variables were extracted from the data: sample size, age, gender composition, country, IQ, primary diagnosis, comorbid diagnoses, medication status, details of diagnostic assessment, measures of inhibitory control, definition of inhibitory control, dependent variable as a measure of inhibitory control, measures of ADHD symptoms, eect size, and confounding variables. Where a consensus could not be reached, a third reviewer was consulted (DS). The
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology
1 3
PRISMA ow diagram outlines assessment of studies in Fig. 1.
Results
Studies Included
The following meta-analysis and synthesis will address differences between a healthy control (HC) group and children with a diagnosis of ODD/CD, ODD/CD + ADHD, or ADHD. In total, 25 studies were considered for meta- analysis (see Table 1), with the total included population
ranging from 3 to 14 years of age. Across the included studies, eight dierent task paradigms were employed to assess cool inhibitory, one hot inhibitory control measure was used, and one type of rating scale. When reporting ADHD symptoms, papers utilised standardised measures, symptom scales, and symptom counts from diagnostic interview schedules. Rating scales were either reported as a T-score, subscale raw score, or an average item score.
Publication Bias and Quality Appraisal
Publication bias was assessed through visual inspection of the funnel plot of standard error in Hedges G, revealing
Records idenfied through database
searching February 2020 (n = 10, 619)
Sc re en
in g
In clu
de d
El ig ib ili ty
Id en
fi ca o
n Addional records idenfied through
reference list checks (n = 3)
Records aer duplicates removed (n = 4298)
Records screened (n = 4298)
Records excluded (n = 4199)
Full-text arcles assessed for eligibility
(n = 99)
Full-text arcles excluded (n=74) Not in English (n=5) Outside 3-17yrs (n=3) No appropriate comparison (n=13) Non-clinical sample ODD/CD (n = 14) DSM-III (n= 10) ASD/II/TBI (n= 1) Did not assess inhibitory control (n=7) Conference paper/dissertaon (n=15) Duplicate sample/paper (n=2) Unable to source data from authors (n=4)
Studies included in qualitave synthesis
(n=25)
Studies included in quantave synthesis
(meta-analysis) (n = 25)
Fig. 1 PRISMA ow chart of studies included in the review
Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology
1 3
Ta bl
e 1
St ud
y c ha
ra cte
ris tic
s
Re fer
en ce
Su bj
ec ts
Ag e a
M (S
D) Se
x Di
ag no
sti c
cr ite
ria an
d as
se ssm
en t
Re po
rte d
Co
m or
bi d
di
ag no
se s
AD HD
S ym
pt om
M
ea su
re M
ed ica
tio n
sta tu
s Ta
sk
ch ar
ac ter
ist ics
Al br
ec ht
et al
. ( 2
00 5)
HC =
11 OD
D/ CD
= 8
AD HD
= 10
AD HD
+ O
DD /
CD =
11
13 0.8
(1 8.9
) 13
1.5 (2
7.4 )
13 0.1
(1 8.0
) 12
3.7 (1
8.5 )
*r ep
or ted
in
m on
th s
Al l m
ale s
IC D-
10 ; v
er i
ed
by bo
ar d c
er ti
ed
ps yc
hi atr
ist s
Cl in
ica l g
ro up
s: Re
ad in
g a nd
/o r
sp ell
in g
di
so rd
er s
En ur
es is
En co
pr es
is HC
: N on
e
Ch ild
B eh
av io
ur
Ch ec
kl ist
– Pa
re nt
R ep
or t:
At ten
tio n
Pr ob
lem s S
ca le.
Re
po rte
d as
T
sc or
es
No t r
ep or
ted St
op si
gn al
tas k;
M
ea su
re of
re
sp on
se in
hi bi
tio n
us in
g SS
RT e
an d S
to p F
ail ur
e Re
ac tio
n T im
e (R
T) An
to ni
ni et
al .
(2 01
5) HC
= 30
AD HD
+ O
DD =
33 AD
HD -O
DD =
67
9.0 0(
1.8 0)
9.4 4(
1.7 5)
8.8 8(
1.4 8)
20 M
10 F
24 M
9F 50
M 17
F
DS M
-IV ; K
id di
e Sc
he du
le fo
r A
ec tiv
e Di
so rd
er s a
nd
Sc hi
zo ph
re ni
a fo
r S ch
oo l-
Ag e C
hi ld
re n
– P re
se nt
an d
Li fet
im e V
er sio
n (K
-S AD
S- PL
)
Cl in
ica l g
ro up
s: Sp
ec i
c p ho
bi as
an
d s ep
ar ati
on
an xi
ety
K- SA
DS : R
ep or
ted
as sy
m pt
om
co un
t
Ch ild
re n w
er e
ex clu
de d i
f t he
y we
re ta
ki ng
an
y p sy
ch iat
ric
m ed
ica tio
n
Co m
pu ter
ize d B
er g
Ca rd
S or
tin g T
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.
