Instructions Chapter 10 discusses diffusion of responsibilitya belief
- Instructions
Chapter 10 discusses diffusion of responsibility—a belief that others will help someone in need, leading to a lessened sense of individual responsibility and a lower probability of helping. In this assignment, you will explore how diffusion of responsibility is exhibited in a real-world setting. To conduct this demonstration, when you are at work, on campus, or in some other public situation, act as if you need help with some minor problem. For example, you can look around confusedly while looking at your phone or drop something that will scatter a bit. Choose something innocuous and harmless to yourself. Do this a couple of times: once when there are several people present and once when there are only one or two people around.
After you complete these actions, write down your notes right away. Using your notes, compose an essay addressing the following points.
- Describe what you did and how it indicated a need for help to others.
- Explain the behavioral response to the situation when many people were present and when only a few people were present, including any differences between the two conditions.
- Discuss whether the response you received fit with the textbook’s discussion of the bystander effect. If your demonstration did not work out, explain why you think it might not have.
- Describe a behavior that may elicit an aggressive, rather than a helping, response. Discuss whether you think the likelihood of an aggressive response would differ when many versus few people were present. Compare this pattern of aggressive responses to helping responses.
- Draw on research from the textbook or another resource to support your answers.
- Your response should be at least two pages in length. You must use at least one source as a reference in your paper. All sources used, including the textbook, must be referenced; paraphrased and quoted material must have accompanying citations. Please format your paper and all citations in accordance with APA guidelines.
Resources
The following resource(s) may help you with this assignment.
PSY 3140, Social Psychology 1
Course Learning Outcomes for Unit VI
Upon completion of this unit, students should be able to:
3. Explain how social psychologists study human behavior. 3.1 Compare motivations underlying helping behaviors versus aggressive behaviors.
5. Analyze the conclusions of empirical research in social psychology. 5.1 Identify the circumstances and motivations that influence helping behaviors.
7. Examine how our own biases influence perceptions of various behaviors. 7.1 Describe how behaviors can be perceived as requiring help. 7.2 Discuss the application of the bystander effect to a social situation.
Course/Unit Learning Outcomes
Learning Activity
3.1
Unit Lesson Chapter 10, pp. 303–312, 315–318, and 320–326 Chapter 11, pp. 333–339, 342–347, and 351–361 Unit VI Scholarly Activity
5.1 Unit Lesson Chapter 10, pp. 303–312, 315–318, and 320–326 Unit VI Scholarly Activity
7.1 Unit Lesson Chapter 10, pp. 303–312, 315–318, and 320–326 Unit VI Scholarly Activity
7.2 Unit Lesson Chapter 10, pp. 303–312, 315–318, and 320–326 Unit VI Scholarly Activity
Reading Assignment
Chapter 10: Helping and Prosocial Behavior, pp. 303–312, 315–318, and 320–326
Chapter 11: Aggression, pp. 333–339, 342–347, and 351–361
Unit Lesson
Helping and Prosocial Behavior
Prosocial behavior is any act done with the intention of benefiting another person or group (Heinzen & Goodfriend, 2019). Altruism is the desire to help another person out of selfless concern for his or her well- being. Researchers who study prosocial behavior and altruism examine whether people are born with these behaviors or learn them. They are also interested in why people help others, even if it does not benefit them. What do you think the difference is between prosocial behavior and altruism? Can you determine how prosocial behavior and altruism are connected?
One explanation as to why people might help others is based on evolutionary psychology. Explaining altruistic behavior is problematic for the theory of evolution because sometimes people act altruistically, even if it might decrease the chance of passing their genes on to the next generation. Some people have died while helping others, but they often do so in order to benefit their family groups more broadly. This, in turn, can ensure that
UNIT VI STUDY GUIDE
Aggression and Prosocial Behavior
PSY 3140, Social Psychology 2
UNIT x STUDY GUIDE
Title certain familial genetic pools are passed on through reproduction. Could this potentially mean that people help even if it means they will not get something in return?
Why do you help others? Is it because you expect to be helped in the future? Recall from Chapter 7 that the norm of reciprocity suggests that we do things to help others with the expectation of an increased likelihood for them to help us in the future (Heinzen & Goodfriend, 2019). According to this norm, sociobiologists propose that when helping nonrelatives, individuals expected that altruistic acts would be returned in kind, which furthered their own survival. Much of what you do can be explained by social exchange theory—the desire to maximize your rewards and to reduce costs, including avoiding or reducing your own negative emotional states. This theory is based on self-interest, but that self-interest does not necessarily have a genetic basis. What are your thoughts about this theory?
If, after reading about social exchanges, you thought it did not reflect true altruism, you are correct. Do you think that people always help others to benefit themselves? Can you think of an example of when you helped to benefit yourself? What about an example of when you helped even though you received nothing in return? Batson (1991) proposes an alternative idea to explain why people help. He suggests that people do help out of the goodness of their hearts because of their ability to experience empathy for a person in need. According to Batson’s empathy-altruism hypothesis, when you feel empathy toward someone, you will help him or her for unselfish reasons. How might you be able to apply Batson’s empathy-altruism hypothesis to the real world in order to increase helping?
Could personality explain the desire to help others? That is, are some personalities more prone to helping others versus other types of personalities? Various personality characteristics have been connected with helping behavior, but consistent evidence is lacking in the consensus of a helping personality trait. Thus, it is unlikely that personality solely determines helping. Other factors that might influence individual differences in helping include religious and cultural upbringings that place value on altruism and helping others in need. Unfortunately, as Darley and Batson (1973) found in their classic Good Samaritan study, these values may not be enough to provide help if people are in a rush or have other things to do. Gender may also contribute to differences in helping behaviors, as gender socialization typically steers males to be more agentic and females to be more communal (Heinzen & Goodfriend, 2019).
If you get hurt, would not you want to be in a large group to increase the odds that someone will help you? Well, according to some social psychologists, you may not. As mentioned in Chapter 8, diffusion of responsibility can influence when people take action. Latané and Darley (1970) were interested in the murder of Kitty Genovese in New York, which occurred on a busy New York street. They hypothesized that the larger number of bystanders present led to the failure to help. To test this hypothesis, they conducted an experiment. Participants sat in separate booths and were asked to communicate over an intercom. On the intercom, the other participant faked having a seizure. The experimenters manipulated participants’ perceptions of how many other people were present during the seizure. What do you think the results were? When
the participants believed more people were present, they were slower to help. This became known as the bystander effect. What would you do if you saw someone who needed help? Do you think that your decision to help would vary based on the number of bystanders present? View the following video clip to learn more about Latané and Darley’s interest and work on the bystander effect and the potential for bystander intervention:
The bystander effect states that the more people that view an event, the less likely people are to help (Corepics Vof, 2010)
PSY 3140, Social Psychology 3
UNIT x STUDY GUIDE
Title
Video Education America (Producer). (2009). Bystander intervention (Custom Segment 14) [Video file]. Retrieved from https://libraryresources.columbiasouthern.edu/login?auth=CAS&url=http://fod.infobase.com/PortalPla ylists.aspx?wID=273866&xtid=129134&loid=507036
The transcript for this video can be found by clicking the “Transcript” tab to the right of the video in the Films on Demand database.
Latané and Darley (1970) also developed a five-step model of helping to explain when people are most likely to help others. You can see a summary of the model depicted in Figure 10.2 in your textbook, but they suggest that a person needs to 1) notice and 2) recognize the event as an emergency first. If there is no emergency, there is no need for aid. Then, a person needs to 3) take responsibility for providing and 4) know how to provide the necessary help. Finally, a person has to 5) decide to actually act on the helping behavior because even if there is need and a person can help, the costs may outweigh the benefits of helping.
Aggression
Distinctive from assertiveness, aggressive action is intentional behavior aimed at doing harm or causing pain to another person who does not want to be harmed (Heinzen & Goodfriend, 2019). Though many people may automatically think of physical acts of aggression when considering the term, the intent to harm aspect allows for a much broader range of behaviors to be included. Aggression can be categorized in a variety of ways, including different aggressive behaviors and triggers of aggression. Hostile/reactive aggression is when someone reacts impulsively to perceived threats. Distinct from this form of aggression, instrumental/proactive aggression involves harming others in order to gain some type of resource. The difference between these forms of aggression is that instrumental/proactive aggression is more thought out and reason-based, though both may act as anchors of an aggression continuum. Despite the existence of aggression at all documented points within human history and the ease in which aggressive responses can escalate, data have shown an overall decline in worldwide violence.
Like prosocial behavior, evolutionary theory has also been used to explain aggression. What do you think evolutionary theory would suggest about aggressive behaviors? Genetic influence alone may not be enough to explain aggression, but some researchers suggest that aggression may be useful in securing higher social status and its accompanying resources, such as the ability to reproduce successfully. Other researchers have found evidence for biological mechanisms across all sexes that contribute to aggressive behaviors, such as engaging in the fight response to threats and higher levels of testosterone (Heinzen & Goodfriend, 2019).
Aggression also can be explained using the context of cultural influences. Are men more aggressive than women are? Research suggests that men engage in physical aggression more often than women do, but gender differences are much smaller when people, of any gender, are provoked (Bettencourt & Miller, 1996). In addition, research indicates that women engage in more verbal aggression (e.g., gossiping, spreading rumors) when compared to men. Therefore, it is important to acknowledge that humans, despite gender, are capable of engaging in aggressive behaviors. It is also important to note that cultures emphasizing gender equality and individualism exhibit less female victimization. In essence, gender roles can dictate how and to whom aggression is expressed. Can you think of other ways cultural forces may influence aggressive tendencies and outcomes?
What else contributes to aggression? Social learning is a major factor for aggression. Noting this, Albert Bandura (as cited in Heinzen & Goodfriend, 2019) developed the social learning theory. Children learn to act aggressively when such behavior is rewarded or socially sanctioned, such that aggression can be learned rather than inherited. In a classic experiment using Bobo dolls, Bandura, Ross, and Ross (1961) showed that children indeed imitated novel aggressive behaviors modeled by adults and other children. To learn more about this study, watch the short video below:
Online Classroom Ltd. (Producer). (2007). Bobo doll experiment (Custom Segment 68) [Video file]. Retrieved from https://libraryresources.columbiasouthern.edu/login?auth=CAS&url=http://fod.infobase.com/PortalPla ylists.aspx?wID=273866&xtid=40125&loid=504404
PSY 3140, Social Psychology 4
UNIT x STUDY GUIDE
Title The transcript for this video can be found by clicking the “Transcript” tab to the right of the video in the Films on Demand database.
Other support for social learning as a cause of aggression comes from long-term studies revealing relationships between watching violence on television as children and exhibiting greater violence later as teens and adults. Notice the usage of the term relationship because causality cannot be determined from these studies. Evidence from experimental research, however, has also revealed that watching violence in the media increases aggression in children. Despite this connection to aggression, media sources can provide positive outcomes, too. For example, in a series of experiments, participants who played a prosocial video game were more likely to help after playing the game than those who played a neutral game (Greitemeyer & Osswald, 2010).
Physical pain increases the likelihood that both animals and humans will act aggressively. Bodily discomfort, like humidity, heat, air pollution, or offensive odors, might also contribute to aggressive behaviors. Why might these factors contribute to aggression? Can you think of any examples from the media in which one of these factors were cited as a reason that someone or a group of people acted aggressively?
Another source of aggression can be frustration. Frustration occurs when you feel like an expected goal or gratification is blocked (Heinzen & Goodfriend, 2019). Frustration is higher when we are close to a goal, and so is the likelihood of aggression, as the aggression functions as an attempt to reclaim what goal is being lost. Usually, it is anger or annoyance that makes you ready to act aggressively, rather than frustration.
The common belief that you can blow off steam and get it (aggression) out of your system is an oversimplification of the catharsis hypothesis (Heinzen & Goodfriend, 2019). The idea is that aggressive thoughts and behaviors are reduced when you purposefully engage in aggressive behaviors (or even watch others act aggressively). Although some psychologists believe strongly in the catharsis hypothesis, controlled studies suggest that acting aggressively or viewing aggression instead increases aggression and hostility, rather than reducing it. Results are similar even when aggression is directed to the source of anger. Thus, there is no support for the catharsis hypothesis or even getting revenge, regardless of how it feels at the time. Instead, committing aggressive acts reduces the barriers toward further aggression and aids in justifying aggressive acts. A better approach to dealing effectively with aggression is to try to establish cultural norms that value peace and model forgiveness when instances of anger and aggression arise.
Prosocial and aggressive behaviors are portrayed as opposites, and at face value, they are. However, when you investigate what drives behavior, both can function similarly. Whether helping or hurting, people are constantly interacting with and responding to others, and they are typically trying to gain something out of the interaction. Some of these tendencies may be biologically driven, but there is strong evidence that we primarily learn both of these tendencies from the social world in which we are raised and live. As with anything, too much of helping or hurting can be detrimental. You do not want to hurt others unnecessarily or too often, but you also do not want others to take advantage of your kindness either. Finding a balance can start by simply taking note of what the social environment needs. By accurately assessing what the situation calls for, we can all be more purposeful and beneficial with our actions.
You have probably experienced frustration at one time or another. Do you think that you would have been more likely to engage in aggression at this time? (Alphaspirit, n.d.)
PSY 3140, Social Psychology 5
UNIT x STUDY GUIDE
Title
References
Alphaspirit. (n.d.). Stress and frustration (ID 38449818) [Photograph]. Retrieved from www.dreamstime.com
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(3), 575–582.
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum.
Bettencourt, B. A., & Miller, N. (1996). Gender differences in aggression as a function of provocation: A meta- analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 119(3), 422–447.
Corepics Vof. (2010). Car crash (ID 16105360) [Photograph]. Retrieved from www.dreamstime.com
Darley, J. M., & Batson, C. D. (1973). “From Jerusalem to Jericho”: A study of situational and dispositional variables in helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27(1), 100–108.
Greitemeyer, T., & Osswald, S. (2010). Effects of prosocial video games on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(2), 211–221.
Heinzen, T., & Goodfriend, W. (2019). Social psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why doesn’t he help? New York, NY: Appleton-Century Crofts.
Suggested Reading
The PowerPoint presentations below serve as a companion to the chapters in this unit. You are encouraged to view them for a deeper understanding of the material presented in this unit.
Click here to view the Chapter 10 PowerPoint Presentation. Click here to view the presentation as a PDF.
Click here to view the Chapter 11 PowerPoint Presentation. Click here to view the presentation as a PDF.
In order to access the following resources, click the links below:
There are many different reasons for aggression, some of which you learned about in this unit. One of those reasons is revenge. Read the article below for a psychological perspective of revenge.
Grobbink, L. H., Derksen, J. J. L., & van Marle, H. J. C. (2015). Revenge: An analysis of its psychological underpinnings. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 59(8), 892– 907. Retrieved from http://journals.sagepub.com/stoken/default+domain/ZUnpCI2ZFpQcueNR2Num/full
In this unit, you learned about the bystander effect. What affect does a camera have on bystander affect? The article below delves into this question.
van Bommel, M., van Prooijen, J.-W., Elffers, H., & van Lange, P. A. M. (2014). Intervene to be seen: The power of a camera in attenuating the bystander affect. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(4), 459–466. Retrieved from http://journals.sagepub.com/stoken/default+domain/IsIGFpfmqNhsJvq9dxYp/full
PSY 3140, Social Psychology 6
UNIT x STUDY GUIDE
Title
Learning Activities (Nongraded)
Nongraded Learning Activities are provided to aid students in their course of study. You do not have to submit them. If you have questions, contact your instructor for further guidance and information.
Test yourself on concepts covered in Chapters 10 and 11. Mastering this material will help you complete the assignment in this unit. Click the links below to view the flashcards and quizzes for each unit.
Click here for the Chapter 10 Flashcards. Click here for the Chapter 10 Quiz.
Click here for the Chapter 11 Flashcards. Click here for the Chapter 11 Quiz.
,
10 |
Helping and Prosocial Behavior |
NBC NewsWire / NBCUniversal / Getty Images
Media Library
CHAPTER 10 Media Library
PREMIUM VIDEO
Ask the Experts
Ask the Experts 10: Catherine Borshuk on Altruism
Social Psychology in Action
Social Psychology in Action 10: Bystander Effect
JOURNAL
Journal 10.1: Searching for the Prosocial Personality
Journal 10.2: Intervene to Be Seen
p.303
Learning Objectives
10.1 Explain several general motives for why helping behaviors occur.
10.2 Analyze individual differences regarding why some people are more likely to help.
10.3 Apply psychological concepts regarding what situational variables lead to more or less helping in different settings.
Core Questions
1. What motivates people to help others, in general?
2. Why do some people help more than others?
3. What circumstances make helping more or less likely?
Mark and Scott Kelly are identical twin astronauts who have devoted their lives to space exploration. In 2012, Scott volunteered to spend a year in space so that scientists could study long-term damages to his body due to space travel—and his twin Mark served as the Earth-bound control condition participant. Both astronauts have sacrificed time with their families and physical health so that other people can learn from their experiences.
Amer Almohibany / AFP / Getty Images
The White Helmets, Syria
“The White Helmets” is a nickname for the Syria Civil Defense organization, a nongovernmental, nonprofit group whose members tried to save lives of civilians in Aleppo, Syria, affected by the war there. According to Raed Al Saleh, the head of the White Helmets, these volunteers had saved over 60,000 lives by 2016—but more than 140 of the White Helmets died while trying to help others. The group was nominated for a Nobel Peace Prize and states that they try to live by the Koran’s words, “To save a life is to save all of humanity.”
Dan Kitwood / Getty Images News / Getty Images
Malala Yousafzai
Malala Yousafzai won the Nobel Peace Prize in 2014. At the age of 12, she stood up to the Taliban in Pakistan when she wrote a blog arguing that all women have a basic right to education. She has been the victim of several assassination attempts and threats, but she refuses to stop advocating for other girls around the world.
What motivates these people—and all of the other self-sacrificing heroes throughout history—to help others?
WHAT MOTIVATES PEOPLE TO HELP OTHERS, IN GENERAL?
LO 10.1: Explain several general motives for why helping behaviors occur.
Ask the Experts Ask the Experts 10: Catherine Borshuk on Altruism |
Have you ever donated blood? Spent a weekend volunteering for Habitat for Humanity? Given money to a charity? Held the door for someone holding a heavy package? If so, you’ve tried to help someone in need. Prosocial behavior is a general term for helping others, either on an individual level (like helping someone who’s lost) or on a group level (like donating to a charity). Helping others is perhaps one of the best parts of a social network, and we’ve all probably experienced the joy of feeling that we’ve made a positive difference in the world.
p.304
However, the social psychology of helping is complicated, just like all of the topics covered in this book. One debate that might never go away is over exactly why people help others. Some people believe that it’s possible for us to exhibit altruism (sometimes called pure altruism), that is, to help others purely out of selfless concern for their well-being (Batson, 1990, 1998). Purely altruistic acts are motivated only by the desire to help—and nothing is expected in return. However, slightly more cynical (or more realistic, depending on your view) people argue that pure altruism is a myth. They argue that prosocial behaviors really stem from egoistic altruism, or helping behaviors done in exchange for some kind of personal benefit.
You might protest at this point, thinking, “But I do help others, expecting nothing in return!” Certainly, when we help a stranger, we might never expect to see that person again. When we give to charity, we are sacrificing those financial resources and things we might enjoy buying for ourselves. But when you engage in prosocial behaviors, do you feel like a better person? Are you happier and more fulfilled? If so—isn’t that a reward? If you don’t help, you might feel guilty or sad—and so, could helping be a selfish way to avoid those negative emotions? These indirect or emotional rewards of helping are part of egoistic altruism.
Social psychology has studied the motivations behind helping and prosocial behaviors in general in an attempt to answer these questions scientifically. So far, the field has offered four major explanations for why we engage in prosocial behaviors: evolutionary benefits to the larger group, social norms, avoiding negative emotions, and empathy (see Table 10.1). Let’s talk about each idea.
The Evolutionary Perspective: Prosocial Behaviors Help Our Groups Survive
Life is a little easier if you have good neighbors. It doesn’t matter whether they lived on the next farm three miles down the road or in the opposite apartment only three steps across the hall. For all social animals, there are evolutionary advantages to being a good neighbor. Among our ancient ancestors, these daily prosocial exchanges probably began over food. If you had killed more meat than you could consume before it got rotten, then you would probably trade away your extra meat for someone else’s excess fruit, grain, or other resource. It’s also possible that at some point, you might have given extra resources to someone in the group without immediately expecting something in return.
■ TABLE 10.1 Four Explanations for Prosocial or Helping Behaviors
p.305
Social exchange refers to the evolution of prosocial trading of resources that strengthens the group. Cosmides and Tooby (1992) describe social exchange between humans as “universal and highly elaborated across all human cultures” (p. 164). We exchange favors with neighbors, exchange money for electronic devices, and even exchange promises when negotiating complex treaties between nations. The advantages of sharing food probably led to other prosocial exchanges such as cooperative hunting, mutual defense, communal childcare, and so on.
Over many generations, the trait of altruism would be naturally selected as one of the constellation of characteristics that made it so advantageous to live in cooperative groups. Selfish loners would be more likely to starve to death, be eaten by prey, or at least be less attractive as sexual and relationship partners. Their genes would slowly be washed out of the gene pool. In contrast, helpful, cooperative, generous altruists would survive by becoming skilled at group living. They would attract strong sexual and relationship partners. Their genes would slowly come to dominate the gene pool (see Nesse, 2001; Van Vugt & Van Lange, 2006). In this view, then, prosocial behaviors offer two advantages: (1) they help individuals survive by promoting opportunities to reproduce and thus pass on one’s genes, and (2) and they help the group survive in times of need (by spreading food around, etc.).
Kinship Selection. We described the advantages of being a good neighbor, but you are probably more likely to loan money to a family member than to your neighbor. Kinship selection refers to the evolutionary urge to favor those with closer genetic relatedness. In his best-known book, On the Origin of Species, Darwin (
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.