For this assignment, you will identify the main concepts and terms learned in this weeks online lectures and textbook readings, and create a fictiona
For this assignment, you will identify the main concepts and terms learned in this week’s online lectures and textbook readings, and create a fictional case study (may not be related to actual individuals).
You will use the following guidelines while writing your case study:
- Background: You need to describe the demographics of individuals involved in the case study such as their age, gender, occupation, education, relationships, and family history.
- The case story: You need to describe a scenario demonstrating a group of individuals engaged in groupthink.
- Analysis of the case: You need to utilize the information learned from the online lectures and text readings to analyze the case study. Be specific in your analysis using supporting evidence from outside sources when needed.
- Recommendations: You need to end the case study with your recommendations or suggestions you would have implemented in such a situation to assist in changing the individuals’ behavior.
Group Development.html
Group Development
The process of group development is dynamic and structured. In the previous example, a problem-solving group was formed to determine what went wrong in the process. The war room group of Dr. Strangelove or: How I Learned to Stop Worrying and Love the Bomb is a good depiction of what happens to a group whose members cannot function in sync with each other. In the movie, the group members did not want to relinquish their own interests and control for the benefit of the group.
There are other real-life examples of similar nuances of group development as in the movie; however, they do not have similar outcomes. For instance, during the early 1960s, tension between the Soviet Union and the United States was high, especially during the Cuban missile crisis and the Bay of Pigs invasion by the United States. However, the invasion was a failure, in part, because the planning was laden with misinformation and groupthink (Janis, 1982).
Several studies have been conducted to determine the influence of groupthink on the decision-making process and the quality of outcomes (Schafer & Crichlow, 2002; Postmes, Spears, & Cihangir, 2001). Although in Dr. Strangelove or: How I Learned to Stop Worrying and Love the Bomb, the dysfunctional process did not allow the group to really become cohesive, it did demonstrate the potential for flawed decisions.
Janis, I. (1982). Groupthink: Psychological studies of policy decisions and fiascoes (2nd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Postmes, T., Spears, R., & Cihangir, S. (2001). Quality of decision making and group norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(6), 918–930.
Schafer, M., & Crichlow, S. (2002). The process-outcome connection in foreign policy decision making: A quantitative study building on groupthink. International Studies Quarterly, 46, 45–68.
,
Stages of Group Development
PSY3011 Social Psychology lab
©2016 South University
2 Stages of Group Development
Group and Organizational Behavior
Stages of Group Development
Dr. Strangelove or: How I Learned to Stop Worrying and Love the Bomb (1964) was a unique movie demonstrating the various aspects of group dynamics, especially dysfunctional group dynamics. The movie was based on a novel written by Peter George and was produced and codirected by Stanley Kubrick.
The story begins with U.S. Air Force Base Commander Jack D. Ripper (played by Sterling Hayden) going mad and sending bombers to drop nuclear warheads over the Soviet Union. This leads to a threat of an accidental nuclear war. The star of the movie is Peter Sellers, who portrays three of the main characters: Dr. Strangelove, Lionel Mandrake (an ex-World War II German scientist with a unique outlook on war and missiles), and President Merkin Muffey.
Lionel Mandrake, after confirming the news of no attack being made on the United States, wants to recall the bombers. However, Ripper refuses to reveal the three-letter code required to recall the bombers.
Desperate to avoid World War III, President Muffley summons Soviet Ambassador Alexander de Sadesky (played by Peter Bull) into the Pentagon war room. Muffley also speaks to the Soviet premiere over the telephone. After a humorous exchange, Muffley agrees to provide the Soviets with data on the flight paths of the errant bombers so that Soviet air defenses can shoot them down, if necessary.
President Muffley becomes aware of a secret weapon, the doomsday device, developed by the Soviets, which has the ability to destroy all life on earth. The Soviets plan to use the weapon in case of any attack on the Soviet Union. When Dr. Strangelove, the president’s advisor, is asked about the weapon’s effect, he admits its obvious flaws.
Thinking about the ways to survive, Dr. Strangelove estimates that U.S. society can survive if a few handpicked individuals remain in underground shelters for 100 years, waiting for the fallout to dissipate. Finally, a full-scale attack on the Soviet Union is planned. The movie ends with one bomber hitting its target and the Soviet Union releasing its doomsday device, which destroys the world.
The main focus of the movie was the lack of group development to resolve this potentially apocalyptic scenario. In this movie, group development seemed to fluctuate between the forming and storming stages of Tuckman's (1965) model. The characters in the movie represented strong personalities who had more influence in their own groups (their direct control) than in the newly formed (war room group). In addition, President Muffley, who was considered the leader of this group, did not have an effective leadership style, which, in this case, should have been an authoritative style.
PSY3011 Social Psychology lab
©2016 South University
3 Stages of Group Development
Group and Organizational Behavior
© 2016 South University
,
Group and Organizational Behavior.html
Group and Organizational Behavior
A group can be defined as individual interacting together for a purpose.
When discussing organizational group behavior, the first question that arises is, what is the purpose of a work group, especially a group related to product development, quality improvement, or management? The probable answer to this question is, "The fundamental activity of groups is to integrate individual knowledge into collective knowledge" (Okhuysen & Eisenhardt, 2002, p. 370). Therefore, most work groups entail some sort of change (in the product line, delivery of services, quality of work, work or task design, marketing strategy, or organizational mission or values). These changes enhance group participation and therefore improve group performance and the quality of products. However, sometimes, significant changes in the work process of a group or an organization evoke increased negative job stress, resulting in poor overall performance (Seel, 2001).
See the linked document for an example of group dynamics.
Okhuysen, G., & Eisenhardt, K. (2002). Integrating knowledge in groups: How formal interventions enable flexibility. Organizational Science, 13(4), 370–386.
Additional Materials
View the PDF transcript for Stages of Group Development
media/week6/SUO_PSY3011 Stages of Group Development.pdf
Stages of Group Development
PSY3011 Social Psychology lab
©2016 South University
2 Stages of Group Development
Group and Organizational Behavior
Stages of Group Development
Dr. Strangelove or: How I Learned to Stop Worrying and Love the Bomb (1964) was a unique movie demonstrating the various aspects of group dynamics, especially dysfunctional group dynamics. The movie was based on a novel written by Peter George and was produced and codirected by Stanley Kubrick.
The story begins with U.S. Air Force Base Commander Jack D. Ripper (played by Sterling Hayden) going mad and sending bombers to drop nuclear warheads over the Soviet Union. This leads to a threat of an accidental nuclear war. The star of the movie is Peter Sellers, who portrays three of the main characters: Dr. Strangelove, Lionel Mandrake (an ex-World War II German scientist with a unique outlook on war and missiles), and President Merkin Muffey.
Lionel Mandrake, after confirming the news of no attack being made on the United States, wants to recall the bombers. However, Ripper refuses to reveal the three-letter code required to recall the bombers.
Desperate to avoid World War III, President Muffley summons Soviet Ambassador Alexander de Sadesky (played by Peter Bull) into the Pentagon war room. Muffley also speaks to the Soviet premiere over the telephone. After a humorous exchange, Muffley agrees to provide the Soviets with data on the flight paths of the errant bombers so that Soviet air defenses can shoot them down, if necessary.
President Muffley becomes aware of a secret weapon, the doomsday device, developed by the Soviets, which has the ability to destroy all life on earth. The Soviets plan to use the weapon in case of any attack on the Soviet Union. When Dr. Strangelove, the president’s advisor, is asked about the weapon’s effect, he admits its obvious flaws.
Thinking about the ways to survive, Dr. Strangelove estimates that U.S. society can survive if a few handpicked individuals remain in underground shelters for 100 years, waiting for the fallout to dissipate. Finally, a full-scale attack on the Soviet Union is planned. The movie ends with one bomber hitting its target and the Soviet Union releasing its doomsday device, which destroys the world.
The main focus of the movie was the lack of group development to resolve this potentially apocalyptic scenario. In this movie, group development seemed to fluctuate between the forming and storming stages of Tuckman's (1965) model. The characters in the movie represented strong personalities who had more influence in their own groups (their direct control) than in the newly formed (war room group). In addition, President Muffley, who was considered the leader of this group, did not have an effective leadership style, which, in this case, should have been an authoritative style.
PSY3011 Social Psychology lab
©2016 South University
3 Stages of Group Development
Group and Organizational Behavior
© 2016 South University
,
DEINDIVIDUATION: WHEN DO PEOPLE LOSE THEIR SENSE OF SELF IN GROUPS?
Define “deindividuation” and identify circumstances that trigger it.
In April 2003, in the wake of American troops entering Iraq’s cities, looters—“liberated” from the scrutiny of police—ran rampant. Hospitals lost beds. The National Library lost tens of thousands of old manuscripts and lay in smoldering ruins. Universities lost computers, Page 212chairs, even lightbulbs. The National Museum in Baghdad lost 15,000 precious objects (Burns, 2003a, 2003b; Lawler, 2003c; Polk & Schuster, 2005). “Not since the Spanish conquistadors ravaged the Aztec and Inca cultures has so much been lost so quickly,” reported Science (Lawler, 2003a). “They came in mobs: A group of 50 would come, then would go, and another would come,” explained one university dean (Lawler, 2003b).
Such reports—and those of the 2011 arson and looting that occurred in London, the 2014 looting in Ferguson, Missouri, and the mob sexual assaults in Germany as 2016 dawned—had the rest of the world wondering: What happened to people’s sense of morality? Why did such behavior erupt? And why was it not anticipated?
Deindividuation: During England’s 2011 riots and looting, rioters were disinhibited by social arousal and by the anonymity provided by darkness and their hoods and masks. Later, some of those arrested expressed bewilderment over their own behavior.
©Lewis Whyld/AP Images
Their behavior even left many of the rioters later wondering what had possessed them. In court, some of the arrested London rioters seemed bewildered by their behavior (Smith, 2011). The mother of one of them, a recent university graduate, explained that her daughter had been sobbing in her bedroom since her arrest over a stolen television. “She doesn’t even know why she took it. She doesn’t need a telly.” An engineering student, arrested after looting a supermarket while he was walking home, was said by his lawyer to having “got caught up in the moment” and was now “incredibly ashamed” (Somaiya, 2011).
Doing Together What We Would Not Do Alone
Social facilitation experiments show that groups can arouse people, and social loafing experiments show that groups can diffuse responsibility. When arousal and diffused responsibility combine, and normal inhibitions diminish, the results may be startling. People may commit acts that range from a mild lessening of restraint (throwing food in the dining hall, snarling at a referee, screaming during a rock concert) to impulsive self-gratification (group vandalism, orgies, thefts) to destructive social explosions (police brutality, riots, lynchings).
These unrestrained behaviors have something in common: They are provoked by the power of being in a group. Groups can generate a sense of excitement, of being caught up in something bigger than one’s self. It is hard to imagine a single rock fan screaming deliriously at a private rock concert, or a single rioter setting a car on fire. It’s in group situations that people are more likely to abandon normal restraints, to forget their individual identity, to become responsive to group or crowd norms—in a word, to become what Leon Festinger, Albert Pepitone, and Theodore Newcomb (1952) labeled deindividuated. What circumstances elicit this psychological state?
GROUP SIZE
A group has the power not only to arouse its members but also to render them unidentifiable. The snarling crowd hides the snarling basketball fan. A lynch mob enables its members to believe they will not be prosecuted; they perceive the action as the group’s. Looters, made faceless by the mob, are freed to loot. One researcher analyzed 21 instances in which crowds were present as someone threatened to jump from a building or a bridge (Mann, 1981). When the crowd was small and exposed by daylight, people usually did not try to bait the person with cries of “Jump!” But when a large crowd or the cover of night gave people anonymity, the crowd usually did bait and jeer.
Lynch mobs produce a similar effect: The bigger the mob, the more its members lose self-awareness and become willing to commit atrocities, such as burning, lacerating, or dismembering the victim (Leader et al., 2007; Mullen, 1986a).
In each of these examples, from sports crowds to lynch mobs, evaluation apprehension plummets. People’s attention is focused on the situation, not on themselves. And because “everyone is doing it,” all can attribute their behavior to the situation rather than to their own choices.
Page 213
ANONYMITY
How can we be sure that crowds offer anonymity? We can’t. But we can experiment with anonymity to see if it actually lessens inhibitions. Philip Zimbardo (1970, 2002) got the idea for such an experiment from his undergraduate students, who questioned how good boys in William Golding’s Lord of the Flies could so suddenly become monsters after painting their faces. To experiment with such anonymity, he dressed New York University women in identical white coats and hoods, rather like Ku Klux Klan members (Figure 5). Asked to deliver electric shocks to a woman, they pressed the shock button twice as long as did women who were unconcealed and wearing large name tags. Even dimmed lighting or wearing sunglasses increases people’s perceived anonymity, and thus their willingness to cheat or behave selfishly (Zhong et al., 2010).
FIGURE 5
In Philip Zimbardo’s deindividuation research, anonymous women delivered more shock to helpless victims than did identifiable women.
©Philip Zimbardo
The Internet offers similar anonymity. Millions of those who were aghast at the looting by the Baghdad mobs were on those very days anonymously pirating music tracks using file-sharing software. With so many doing it, and with so little concern about being caught, downloading someone’s copyrighted property and then offloading it to an MP3 player just didn’t seem terribly immoral. Internet bullies who would never say, “Get a life, you phony,” to someone’s face will hide behind their anonymity online. Most social media sites, to their credit, require people to use their real names, which constrains hate-filled comments.
On several occasions, anonymous online bystanders have egged on people threatening suicide, sometimes with live video feeding the scene to scores of people. Online communities “are like the crowd outside the building with the guy on the ledge,” noted one analyst of technology’s social effects (quoted by Stelter, 2008). Sometimes a caring person tried to talk the person down, while others, in effect, chanted, “Jump, jump.” “The anonymous nature of these communities only emboldens the meanness or callousness of the people on these sites.”
Testing deindividuation on the streets, Patricia Ellison, John Govern, and their colleagues (1995) had a driver stop at a red light and wait for 12 seconds whenever she was followed by a convertible or a 4 × 4 vehicle. During the wait, she recorded horn-honking (a mild aggressive act) by the car behind. Compared with drivers of convertibles and 4 × 4s with the car tops down, those who were relatively anonymous (with the tops up) honked one-third sooner, twice as often, and for nearly twice as long. Anonymity feeds incivility.
A research team led by Ed Diener (1976) cleverly demonstrated the effect both of being in a group and of being physically anonymous. At Halloween, they observed 1,352 Seattle children trick-or-treating. As the children, either alone or in groups, approached 1 of 27 homes scattered throughout the city, an experimenter greeted them warmly, invited them to “take one of the candies,” and then left the candy unattended. Hidden observers noted that children in groups were more than twice as likely to take extra candy than were solo children. Also, children who had been asked their names and where they lived were less than half as likely to transgress as those who were left anonymous. As Figure 6 shows, when they were deindividuated both by group immersion and by anonymity, most children stole extra candy.
FIGURE 6
Children were more likely to transgress by taking extra Halloween candy when in a group, when anonymous, and, especially, when deindividuated by the combination of group immersion and anonymity.
Source: Data from Diener et al., 1976.
Page 214Those studies make us wonder about the effect of wearing uniforms. Preparing for battle, warriors in some tribal cultures (like some rabid sports fans) depersonalize themselves with body and face paints or special masks. Robert Watson (1973) scrutinized anthropological files and discovered this: The cultures with depersonalized warriors were also the cultures that brutalized their enemies. In Northern Ireland, 206 of 500 violent attacks studied by Andrew Silke (2003) were conducted by attackers who wore masks, hoods, or other face disguises. Compared with undisguised attackers, these anonymous attackers inflicted more serious injuries, attacked more people, and committed more vandalism.
Does becoming physically anonymous always unleash our worst impulses? Fortunately, no. In all these situations, people were responding to clear antisocial cues. Robert Johnson and Leslie Downing (1979) point out that the Klan-like outfits worn by Zimbardo’s participants may have been stimulus cues for hostility. In an experiment at the University of Georgia, women put on nurses’ uniforms before deciding how much shock someone should receive. When those wearing the nurses’ uniforms were made anonymous, they became less aggressive in administering shocks. From their analysis of 60 deindividuation studies, Tom Postmes and Russell Spears (1998; Reicher et al., 1995) concluded that being anonymous makes one less self-conscious, more group-conscious, and more responsive to situational cues, whether negative (Klan uniforms) or positive (nurses’ uniforms).
AROUSING AND DISTRACTING ACTIVITIES
Aggressive outbursts by large groups are often preceded by minor actions that arouse and divert people’s attention. Group shouting, chanting, clapping, or dancing serve both to hype people up and to reduce self-consciousness.
Experiments have shown that activities such as throwing rocks and group singing can set the stage for more disinhibited behavior (Diener, 1976, 1979). There is a self-reinforcing pleasure in acting impulsively while seeing others do likewise. When we see others act as we are acting, we think they feel as we do, which reinforces our own feelings (Orive, 1984). Moreover, impulsive group action absorbs our attention. When we yell at the referee, we are not thinking about our values; we are reacting to the immediate situation. Later, when we stop to think about what we have done or said, we sometimes feel chagrined. Sometimes. At other times we seek deindividuating group experiences—dances, worship experiences, team sports—where we enjoy intense positive feelings and closeness to others.
“Attending a service in the Gothic cathedral, we have the sensation of being enclosed and steeped in an integral universe, and of losing a prickly sense of self in the community of worshipers.”
—Yi-Fu Tuan, Segmented Worlds and Self, 1982
Diminished Self-Awareness
Group experiences that diminish self-consciousness tend to disconnect behavior from attitudes. Research by Ed Diener (1980) and Steven Prentice-Dunn and Ronald Rogers (1980, 1989) Page 215revealed that unself-conscious, deindividuated people are less restrained, less self-regulated, more likely to act without thinking about their own values, and more responsive to the situation. These findings complement and reinforce the experiments on self-awareness.
Self-awareness is the opposite of deindividuation. Those made self-aware, by acting in front of a mirror or a TV camera, exhibit increased self-control, and their actions more clearly reflect their attitudes. In front of a mirror, people taste-testing cream cheese varieties ate less of the high-fat variety (Sentyrz & Bushman, 1998).
Looking in a mirror or being on camera increases self-awareness, making us think about our individual actions more carefully.
©Syda Productions/Shutterstock
People made self-aware are also less likely to cheat (Beaman et al., 1979; Diener & Wallbom, 1976). So are those who generally have a strong sense of themselves as distinct and independent (Nadler et al., 1982). In Japan, where people more often imagine how they might look to others, the presence of a mirror had no effect on cheating (Heine et al., 2008). The principle: People who are self-conscious, or who are temporarily made so, exhibit greater consistency between their words outside a situation and their deeds in it.
We can apply those findings to many situations in everyday life. Circumstances that decrease self-awareness, as alcohol consumption does, increase deindividuation (Hull et al., 1983). Deindividuation decreases in circumstances that increase self-awareness: mirrors and cameras, small towns, bright lights, large name tags, undistracted quiet, individual clothes and houses (Ickes et al., 1978). When a teenager leaves for a party, a parent’s parting advice could well be “Have fun, and remember who you are.” In other words, enjoy being with the group, but be self-aware; maintain your personal identity; be wary of deindividuation.
SUMMING UP: Deindividuation: When Do People Lose Their Sense of Self in Groups?
When high levels of social arousal combine with diffused responsibility, people may abandon their normal restraints and lose their sense of individuality.
Such deindividuation is especially likely when people are in a large group, are physically anonymous, and are aroused and distracted.
The resulting diminished self-awareness and self-restraint tend to increase people’s responsiveness to the immediate situation, be it negative or positive. Deindividuation is less likely when self-awareness is high.
GROUP POLARIZATION: DO GROUPS INTENSIFY OUR OPINIONS?
Describe and explain how interaction with like-minded people tends to amplify preexisting attitudes.
Do group interactions more often have good or bad outcomes? Police brutality and mob violence demonstrate the destructive potential of groups. Yet support-group leaders, work-group consultants, and educational theorists proclaim the beneficial effects of group interaction. And self-help group members and religious adherents strengthen their identities by fellowship with like-minded others.
Studies of small groups have produced a principle that helps explain both bad and good outcomes: Group discussion often strengthens members’ initial inclinations. The unfolding Page 216of this research on group polarization illustrates the process of inquiry—how an interesting discovery often leads researchers to hasty and erroneous conclusions, which get replaced with more accurate conclusions. This is a scientific mystery I [DM] can discuss firsthand, having been one of the detectives.
The Case of the “Risky Shift”
More than 300 studies began with a surprising finding by James Stoner (1961), then an MIT graduate student. For his master’s thesis in management, Stoner tested the commonly held belief that groups are more cautious than individuals. He posed decision dilemmas in which the participant’s task was to advise imagined characters how much risk to take. Put yourself in the participant’s shoes: What advice would you give the character in this situation?1
Helen is a writer who is said to have considerable creative talent but who so far has been earning a comfortable living by writing cheap westerns. Recently she has come up with an idea for a potentially significant novel. If it could be written and accepted, it might have considerable literary impact and be a big boost to her career. On the other hand, if she cannot work out her idea or if the novel is a flop, she will have expended considerable time and energy without remuneration.
Imagine that you are advising Helen. Please check the lowest probability that you would consider acceptable for Helen to attempt to write the novel.
Helen should attempt to write the novel if the chances that the novel will be a success are at least
_____ 1 in 10
_____ 2 in 10
_____ 3 in 10
_____ 4 in 10
_____ 5 in 10
_____ 6 in 10
_____ 7 in 10
_____ 8 in 10
_____ 9 in 10
_____ 10 in 10 (Place a check here if you think Helen should attempt the novel only if it is certain that the novel will be a success.)
After making your decision, guess what this book’s average reader would advise.
Having marked their advice on a dozen items, five or so individuals would then discuss and reach agreement on each item. How do you think the group decisions compared with the average decision before the discussions? Would the groups be likely to take greater risks, be more cautious, or stay the same?
To everyone’s amazement, the group decisions were usually riskier. This “risky shift phenomenon” set off a wave of group risk-taking studies. These revealed that risky shift occurs not only when a group decides by consensus; after a brief discussion, individuals, too, will alter their decisions. What is more, researchers successfully repeated Stoner’s finding with people of varying ages and occupations in a dozen nations.
The risky shift: Groups of people, like these teens in a car together, may make more risky decisions than individuals alone.
©Big Cheese Photo/Superstock
During discussion, opinions converged. Curiously, however, the point toward which they converged was usually a lower (riskier) number than their initial average. Here was an intriguing puzzle. The small risky shift effect was reliable, unexpected, and without any immediately obvious explanation. What group influences produce such an effect? And how widespread is it? Do discussions in juries, business committees, and military organizations also promote risk taking? Does this explain why teenage reckless driving, as measured by death rates, nearly doubles when a 16- or 17-year-old driver has two teenage Page 217passengers rather than none (Chen et al., 2000)? Does it explain stock bubbles, as people discuss why stocks are rising, thus creating an informational cascade that drives stocks even higher (Sunstein, 2009)?
After several years of study, my [DM’s] colleagues and I discovered that the risky shift was not universal. We could write decision dilemmas on which people became more cautious after discussion. One of these featured “Roger,” a young married man with two school-age children and a secure but low-paying job. Roger can afford life’s necessities but few of its luxuries. He hears that the stock of a relatively unknown company may soon triple in value if its new product is favorably received or decline considerably if it does not sell. Roger has no savings. To invest in the company, he is considering selling his life insurance policy.
Can you see a general principle that predicts both the tendency to give riskier advice after discussing Helen’s situation and more cautious advice after discussing Roger’s? If you are like most people, you would advise Helen to take a greater risk than Roger, even before talking with others. It turns out there is a strong tendency for discussion to accentuate these initial leanings. Thus, groups discussing the “Roger” dilemma became more risk-averse than they were before discussion (Myers, 2010).
Do Groups Intensify Opinions?
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.