Research, read and analyze 2-3 Peer Reviewed articles about a topic of your choice that is related to understanding and working w
Resources: https://www.learningforjustice.org/magazine/fall-2010/helping-the-homeless-in-school-and-out
Requirements:
● Double-spaced and between 5 pages long – not counting your cover and reference page. Please number your pages.
● Research, read and analyze 2-3 Peer Reviewed articles about a topic of your choice that is related to understanding and working with diverse families and/or special student populations.
Topic: Homeless children
Resources: https://www.learningforjustice.org/magazine/fall-2010/helping-the-homeless-in-school-and-out
Requirements:
● Double-spaced and between 5 pages long – not counting your cover and reference page. Please number your pages.
● Research, read and analyze 2-3 Peer Reviewed articles about a topic of your choice that is related to understanding and working with diverse families and/or special student populations.
Cite your sources. Always use parenthetical, in-text citations to reference all of the source material used. All ideas or research that is from an outside source must be cited in APA format
Suggested Format for Response Paper:
1. Introduction
2. Research Findings
3. Discussion
4. Conclusion
5. References (according to APA style)
Introduction:
The introductory paragraph should set the tone for the paper. Give the reader your claim and set up the structure of the paper so that the reader knows what to expect. You may also want to tell your reader why this question is relevant (example of question: What types of resources and supports are available for LGBTQ Parenting?). Why is this topic important?
Research Findings:
The objective of this portion of the paper is to explain the research thoroughly enough to allow your audience to understand the material without having to do any additional reading.
Discussion:
Avoid repeating what you showed in the “Findings” section. Use this section to provide us with your own analysis, opinions, concerns, or questions for further research. This is where you respond to the research.
Conclusion:
Restate your answer to your research question, hypothesis, or primary claim based on your findings.
References:
Your reference page should follow the guidelines of APA Style.
,
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 31 (2010) 109–117
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
Effortful control and adaptive functioning of homeless children: Variable-focused and person-focused analyses
Jelena Obradović ⁎ Stanford University, School of Education, 485 Lasuen Mall, Stanford, CA 94305-3096, United States
⁎ Tel.: +1 650 725 1250; fax: +1 650 725 7412. E-mail address: [email protected]
0193-3973/$ – see front matter © 2009 Elsevier Inc. Al doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2009.09.004
a b s t r a c t
a r t i c l e i n f o
Article history: Received 29 October 2008 Received in revised form 22 September 2009 Accepted 29 September 2009 Available online 27 November 2009
Keywords: Homeless children Effortful control Executive functions Adaptation Risk Resilience
Homeless children show significant developmental delays across major domains of adaptation, yet research on protective processes that may contribute to resilient adaptation in this highly disadvantaged group of children is extremely rare. This study examined the role of effortful control for adaption in 58 homeless children, ages 5–6, during their transition to school. Effortful control skills were assessed using children's performance on four standard laboratory tasks. Adaptive functioning was assessed by teacher report of academic competence, peer competence, and internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Variable-focused and person-focused results indicate that effortful control may be an important marker of school readiness and resilience. Controlling for child IQ, parenting quality, and socio-demographic risks, effortful control emerged as the most significant predictor of all four salient developmental domains of adaptation as well as of resilient status of homeless children. Implications of these findings are discussed for future research and design of interventions.
l rights reserved.
© 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Introduction
Twenty years of research on homeless children and families has produced a substantial body of literature showing that homeless children living in poverty are at a very high risk for delays in multiple domains of adaptive functioning, including academic, social, emo- tional, and behavioral problems (Buckner, 2008; Buckner, Bassuk, Weinreb, & Brooks, 1999; Haber & Toro, 2004; Masten, 1992; Masten, Miliotis, Graham-Bermann, Ramirez, & Neemann, 1993; Rafferty & Shinn, 1991). In addition to the cumulative risks associated with poverty, such as traumatic life experiences, parental psychopathology, and lack of support systems (Luthar, 1999; McLoyd, Aikens, & Burton, 2006), homeless children face specific threats to development from residential instability and broken bonds with potentially positive sources of security and opportunity (Rafferty, Shinn, & Weitzman, 2004; Rog & Buckner, 2007). However, recent studies have demon- strated that homeless children are not a homogenous group (Huntington, Buckner, & Bassuk, 2008; Obradović et al., 2009). Evidence of positive adaptation in homeless children, even at very high levels of risk, suggests that there is significant variability in the promotive and protective factors influencing the lives of these children. Understanding the processes that support successful adaptation in children exposed to homelessness and its attendant risks are important for preventive intervention. And yet, research on
positive processes that contribute to resilient adaption in this highly disadvantaged group of children is rare. The current study was designed to fill this gap by examining how effortful control abilities relate to multiple domains of adaptive functioning in 5 and 6-year-old homeless children during their transition to school.
Adaptive functioning in homeless children
Homeless children fall at the high end of the risk continuum of children living in poverty, showing significant developmental delays across the major domains of adaptation. Studies of academic success have found that a large majority of homeless children show severe academic achievement delays and perform below grade level norms (Masten et al., 1997; Rubin et al., 1996; Zima, Wells, & Freeman, 1994). Masten et al. (1993) found that a significantly higher percentage of homeless children have clinical levels of social problems when compared to national norms. Moreover, homeless children show elevated levels of externalizing and internalizing behavior problems, often above clinical thresholds (Masten et al., 1997).
Further, many studies have shown that homeless children present a unique risk group that is distinct from the larger group of children living in poverty. When compared to economically disadvantaged children living at home, homeless children show lower levels of literacy and arithmetic skills (Rubin et al., 1996; Obradović et al., 2009), are more likely to report having no close friends (Masten et al., 1997), and are at higher risk for clinical levels of psychopathology (Masten et al., 1993; Rescorla, Parker, & Stolley, 1991; Vostanis, Grattan, Cumella & Winchester, 1997). These differences often remain
110 J. Obradović / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 31 (2010) 109–117
after controlling for important confounds, such as race, social class, and family composition (Rubin et al., 1996).
Nevertheless, housing status has been an inconsistent predictor of adaptation (Buckner, 2008). Homeless children tend to face higher levels of adversity exposure, and housing status per se might not be the most important marker of risk. For example, Buckner et al. (1999) found that housing status predicted internalizing symptoms but not externalizing symptoms, controlling for the significant contribution of age, ethnicity, maternal distress, stressful life events, and history of abuse. However, homeless children had significantly higher levels of stressful life events and history of sexual abuse. Similarly, Masten et al. (1993) reported that housing status did not predict overall symptom levels over and above parental distress, demographic risk, and negative life events, but homeless children on average were exposed to nearly twice as many negative life events. In a recent review of studies on homeless children, Buckner (2008) argued that incon- sistencies in the literature may be largely due to historical, contextual, and policy-related factors. For example, he suggests that recent studies reveal fewer differences between homeless and low-income, housed children in part due to better social policies and services currently in place for homeless families that did not exist in the 1980s. More importantly, Buckner (2008) advised that additional studies examining differences between homeless and housed children may not meaningfully advance our understanding of what can improve homeless children's adaption.
While homelessness presents a marker of high cumulative risk, there are hints of considerable variation in the adaptation of homeless children, with some children showing signs of positive development (Obradović et al., 2009). However, little is known about the protective factors that may account for resilient functioning among homeless children. In one of the few studies focused on protective factors, Masten and colleagues identified closeness of the parent–child relationship and parental involvement in education as significant predictors of homeless children’s school success (Masten & Sesma, 1999; Milotis, Sesma & Masten, 1999). More research is needed to identify other protective processes that contribute to the variability of adaption in homeless children and to differentiate subgroups of children who do well despite adversity.
As Buckner (2008) points out, the next generation of studies on homeless children needs to have clear implications for policy and intervention program designs. One key focus of this research should be early childhood, as developmental delays appear to emerge early and persist over time. In a recent study of achievement trajectories in a large urban school district, homeless and highly mobile students showed lower initial levels of achievement than low-income classmates who were more stably housed as early as second grade, controlling for sex, ethnicity, English language proficiency, and attendance (Obradović et al., 2009). Moreover, this gap persisted over three school years, underscoring the importance of examining factors that may prevent early school failures in homeless children. School entry may present a particularly vulnerable time for homeless children, as residential instability may jeopardize the consistency of their educational experiences and interfere with the formation of new peer groups.
Effortful control and adaptive functioning
Scientists examining processes that promote successful develop- ment in high-risk children have emphasized the importance of investigating systems that (a) bridge multiple levels of analysis, (b) are implicated in the salient domains of adaptation, and (c) are amenable to change (Luthar, 2006; Masten & Obradović, 2006). One system that meets all three criteria is effortful control, a set of executive functions aimed at the intentional, internal manipulation of one's attention and behavior. Effortful control is frequently defined as the ability to inhibit a dominant response in order to execute a
subdominant response (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). It includes skills such as inhibitory control, attention shifting, and attention focusing. Together, these abilities play an important role in achieving the developmental tasks of the school years: learning, forming friend- ships, and following the rules of classroom and society.
Effortful control has been linked to multiple domains of adaptive functioning. Recent studies show the significant influence of attention regulation and inhibitory control on early academic competence, such as reading and math achievement in preschoolers and kindergarten- ers (Blair & Razza, 2007; Howse, Lange, Farran, & Boyles, 2003; Senn, Espy, & Kaufmann, 2004). Effortful control has also been linked to social competence in community samples of 8 to 12-year-olds (Lengua, 2002, 2003) and preschool children (Lengua, Honorado, & Bush, 2007). Further, a robust relation has been documented between effortful control skills and externalizing behavior problems in preschoolers (Eisenberg et al., 1997; Kochanska & Knaack, 2003; Olson, Sameroff, Kerr, Lopez, & Wellman, 2005), and this relation persists even when the high longitudinal stability and concurrent intercorrelations of both constructs are taken into account (Valiente et al., 2003; Eisenberg et al., 2004). Initial levels as well as improvements in effortful control skills have been found to predict short-term changes in externalizing and internalizing behavior problems (Eisenberg et al., 2005; Riggs, Blair, & Greenberg, 2003). Together, these studies suggest that in community samples effortful control can be a powerful correlate and predictor of adaptation across the salient developmental domains of childhood. However, most of the research on effortful control and adaptive functioning has focused on Caucasian, middle-class samples (Eisenberg, Hofer, & Vaughan, 2007).
Development of effortful control is shaped by both biological and environmental factors (Eisenberg et al., 2003; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2005; Rothbart, Sheese, & Posner, 2007; Tarullo, Obradović & Gunnar, 2009). Recent studies suggest that risk and adversity exposure significantly undermine effortful control. Eco- nomically disadvantaged 5 to 7-year-olds performed significantly worse on attention regulation and attention shifting tasks than their more affluent age-mates (Howse et al, 2003, Mezzacappa, 2004). In a community sample of preschoolers, indices of poverty, mobility, and family problems were related to lower performance on effortful control tasks, and a cumulative index of demographic and psychoso- cial risks negatively predicted 6-month change in effortful control (Lengua et al., 2007). Similarly, Li-Grining (2007) reported that socio- demographic and residential risks negatively predicted effortful control in low-income preschoolers.
Given that risk and adversity exposure undermine effortful control skills, it is surprising that few studies have examined how effortful control contributes to adaptive functioning in at-risk children. In the Head Start sample, Blair and colleagues found some evidence of relation between performance on effortful control tasks and teacher report of classroom behaviors (Blair, Granger, & Razza, 2005; Blair & Peters, 2003). In another study, effortful control tasks predicted math and literacy skills in kindergarten (Blair & Razza, 2007). Although these studies provide initial evidence that effortful control is important for the adaptation of economically disadvantaged children, the researchers did not control for family sources of risk and resources. Moreover, homeless children living in an emergency shelter tend to face higher levels of adversity than low-income housed children, which may further undermine the association between effortful control and adaptation.
One published study to date has examined self-regulation in children living in extreme poverty and exposed to high adversity, including homelessness. Buckner, Mezzacappa, and Beardslee (2003) showed that self-regulation differentiated groups of children who showed resilient and maladaptive adaptation and was also a significant predictor of a continuous measure of resilience, controlling for important confounds such as negative life events, chronic strains,
111J. Obradović / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 31 (2010) 109–117
abuse history, IQ, self-esteem, parental monitoring, and emotional support. While this study established the importance of studying self- regulation in highly disadvantaged children, the self-regulation construct combined ratings of effortful control skills with measures of reactivity, motivation, and emotion regulation; thus, the distinct contributions of specific elements were not evident. Moreover, children's ages varied considerably from 8 to 17 years, making it difficult to assess the effects of effortful control in early childhood, especially during the transition to school.
Current study
The current study was designed to examine the variability of functioning within the homeless children population (rather than comparing it to low-income housed children) and to identify processes that promote homeless children’s positive adaptation. It aims to extend the findings of Buckner and colleagues (2003) by testing the effect of effortful control skills, as measured by laboratory tasks, on various domains of adaptation during an important developmental transition period. At age 5 to 6, children show significant developmental improvements in effortful control, just as they begin to face new challenges in the school environment. As advocated by Buckner (2008), this study incorporated both variable- focused and person-focused analytic approaches. The first goal was to examine the separate relations between effortful control and four salient developmental domains of adaption. The second goal was to examine whether effortful control identifies a group of children who demonstrate resilience across all four domains of adaptation, as indexed by average or better functioning. In accordance with existing literature, effortful control was expected to relate to indices of academic achievement, peer competence, and internalizing and externalizing symptoms, and to differentiate resilient from maladap- tive homeless children. All analyses were tested controlling for sex and age. In addition, the unique effects of effortful control were examined over and above child IQ, parenting quality, and family socio-demographic risks, three key correlates of adaptation among both low- and high-risk samples.
Methods
Participants and procedures
Participants for this study were recruited from one of the largest homeless shelters in the upper Midwest. Children in this shelter represent approximately 40% of all children living in homeless shelters who enroll at any point during the school year in the urban public school district in which it is located. Families with children who were scheduled to enter kindergarten or first grade in the fall of 2006 were invited to participate in the study. Due to the nature of the assessments, families who did not speak English were not eligible for the study, and five families were excluded on this basis. As the study progressed, it became evident that the current census at the shelter had more eligible boys and more first graders; thus, if a family had two or more eligible children, females and kindergartners were recruited.
The sample consisted of 58 children (20 females, 38 male). The mean age of the children was 6.09 (SD = 0.54 range 4.97–7.23). The sample included 81.0% African-American, 1.7% American Indian, 3.4% Anglo- American, and 13.8% mixed-ethnicity children. The ethnic composition of this sample is very similar to previous studies of homeless children living in the same city (Masten et al., 1993, 1997). About 75% of children lived with a single caregiver; 90% of primary caregivers were mothers, 7% were fathers, and 3% were grandmothers. For simplicity, from this point forward primary caregivers are referred to as parents.
Although an effort was made to schedule sessions at least two days after the family's arrival at the shelter, in order to allow them to acclimate to the new environment, one family participated in the
study before their second night at the shelter. The duration of homelessness is difficult to estimate, as many families live “doubled up” with their relatives or friends prior to arriving at the shelter, while others bounce between different emergency shelters. At the time of participation, the parent-reported length of families' current shelter stay ranged from 1 to 365 days (Mdn = 9 days; M = 26; SD = 64). Forty-three percent of parents reported being without their own housing (rented or owned by the parent or a partner) for less than a month, 22% for 1–3 months, 19% for 3 months to 1 year, 14% for more than a year, and one family never had their own housing. Moreover, 46.6% of parents reported being homeless at other times in their lives. Due to the nature of homelessness, it was not feasible to determine when these families ended being homeless.
Children and parents separately participated in a 90-minute session conducted on shelter premises. Children completed a series of standardized intelligence subtests and a battery of effortful control tasks, while parents were interviewed and administered various questionnaires. In the fall of 2006, 22 of the children entered kindergarten and 36 entered the first grade. Teachers of the 54 children who attended area schools were invited to complete a questionnaire about the child's adaptive functioning, and 100% of the teachers returned the questionnaire, representing 93% of the original sample. On average, teacher completed their assessments approxi- mately 4 months after the testing session was conducted at the shelter (range: 20–229 days; M = 115, SD = 52).
It is difficult to ascertain participation rates accurately for studies in emergency shelters, because the families continuously move in and out. At the outset, some families move out before they learn of the study and others move before they can be scheduled to participate. Based on participation rates during the middle week of each of the three full months when assessments were conducted, approximately 90% of families with eligible children participated in the study, suggesting a highly representative sample of families residing in this emergency shelter.
Measures
Effortful control Children's effortful control skills were assessed using a battery of
four standard laboratory tasks that were selected based on their developmental appropriateness and ability to tap different aspects of effortful control (Carlson, 2005). In the Simon Says task (Strommen, 1973; Kochanska, Murray, & Coy, 1997) children were instructed to imitate the experimenter's behavior during commands prefaced by the words “Simon says”, while inhibiting imitations when commands were not prefaced with “Simon says”. After practice trials, children were administered a series of 20 counterbalanced trials (10 activation “Simon says” trials and 10 inhibition trials). Two trained coders, blind to children's performance on all other effortful control tasks as well as all other family or child data, coded the degree of transgression or movement during the 10 inhibition trials. Interrater reliability for inhibitory trials was calculated based on 25% of the sample (weighted κ = .94). There was no variability in activation scores as all children performed well on activation trials. In the Peg-tapping task (Diamond & Taylor, 1996), children were instructed to tap twice with a wooden dowel when the experimenter tapped once and to tap once when the experimenter tapped twice. After practice trials, the children were presented with 16 counterbalanced test trials. Performance on the Peg-tapping task was measured by the number of correct taps on a scale from 0 to 16 which were recorded during the task. In the computerized pointing Stroop task (Berger, Jones, Rothbart, & Posner, 2000), children were trained first to point to a picture matching an animal sound (e.g., congruent trials: point to the picture of a cat after hearing ‘meow’), and then to point to a picture not matching the animal sound (e.g., incongruent trials: point to the dog after hearing ‘meow’). After practice trials, children were presented with a series of
112 J. Obradović / Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 31 (2010) 109–117
16 congruent trials followed by a series of 16 incongruent trials. Performance on incongruent Stroop trials was recorded by computer software on a 0 to 16 scale. Two children performed at or below chance on congruent trials, indicating that they did not understand the task, and their incongruent trials were not analyzed. During the Dimensional Change Card Sort task (DCCS; Zelazo, 2006), children were taught first to play the “color game”, placing all red cards in a box with a red rabbit and all blue cards in a box with a blue boat. After they learned this rule, they were asked to play the “shape game”, placing all cards with blue or red rabbits in the box with the red rabbit and all cards with blue or red boats in the box with the blue boat, which required them to inhibit the previously established pattern of sorting by color. DCCS consisted of 6 “color game” trials before the rule switch and 6 “shape game” trials after the rule switch. Performance was scored during the task on a scale from 0 to 6, reflecting the number of correctly sorted cards during the post-switch, “shape game” trials.
The number of correct trials on the Simon Says task, Peg-tapping task, and Stroop task were transformed into percentage scores to account for any missing data due to procedural irregularities (e.g., the child was off camera during one Simon Says trial). A composite index of effortful control skills (EC) was calculated by averaging standard- ized scores on the four effortful control tasks. The four effortful control tasks scores satisfied Patterson and Bank's (1986) criteria by showing high internal consistency as a scale (α = .75), high item-total correlations (r N .70, p b .001), and loading on a single factor with all loadings over .70.
Adaptive functioning Four different domains of children's adaptive functioning were
assessed using the teacher form of The MacArthur Health Behavioral Questionnaire (HBQ; Armstrong, Goldstein, & The MacArthur Working Group on Outcome Assessment, 2003). All the subscale and scale reliability statistics are presented for this sample. Academic Compe- tence was assessed using the HBQ Academic Functioning Scale, which is an average of two subscales (r = .33, p b .05): (1) the eight-item School Engagement subscale (α = .87) and (2) the five- item Academic Competence subscale (α = .96). Peer Competence was assessed using the HBQ Peer Relations Scale, which is an average of two subscales (r = .63, p b .001): (1) the eight-item Peer Acceptance/ Rejection subscale (α = .94) and (2) the three-item Bullied by Peers subscale (α = .80). Internalizing Behavioral Problems was assessed using the HBQ Internalizing Symptoms Scale, which is an average of two subscales (r = .70, p b .001): (1) the six-item Depression subscale (α = .83) and (2) the eight-item Overanxious subscale (α = .69). Externalizing Behavioral Problems was assessed using the HBQ Externalizing Symptoms Scale, which is an average of four subscales (α = .89): (1) the nine-item Oppositional Defiant subscale (α = .92), (2) the eleven-item Conduct Problem subscale (α = .85), (3) the four- item Overt Hostility subscale (α = .80), and (4) the six-item Relational Aggression subscale (α = .83).
Homeless children were classified as resilient if they scored all of the following: (1) 3 or higher on a 5-point academic functioning scale, which corresponds to average academic competence and some school engagement, (2) 3 or higher on a 4-point peer competence scale, which corresponds to somewhat likely to be accepted and not very likely to be rejected and bullied by peers, (3) lower than the clinical threshold for the depression subscale (b .86) and anxiety subscale (b .91), and (4) lower than the clinical threshold for the oppositional defiant subscale (b .84) and conduct problem subscale (b .79). Clinical thresholds were determined by Luby and colleagues (2002), who evaluated perfor- mance of parent-reported HBQ symptoms in identifying DSM-IV internalizing and externalizing symptoms in referred and non-referred 4 to 8-year-olds. Clinical thresholds derived by Luby et al. (2002) were employed over the more recent Lemery-Chalfant et al. (2007) thresh- olds because the latter study focused on children older than the current sample and did not provide subscale level data for internalizing
symptoms. However, classifications based on the two set of thresholds overlap considerably (93% for externalizing symptoms).
Risk and resources A Cumulative Risk Index was created by summing five well-
established socio-demographic risk factors (M = 1.94; SD = 1.10): single parent household (74%), maternal age at first birth younger than 18 years old (37%), 3 or more siblings living with the family (33%), primary caregiver's education less than a high school degree (26%), and no current income (27%). Continuous risk factors (i.e., maternal age) were dichotomized according to meaningful cutpoint standards com- monly applied in risk studies (Sameroff, Seifer, Zax, & Barocas, 1987).
Children's General Intellectual Functioning (IQ) was measured using the Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, and Vocabulary subtests of the Wechsler Preschool and the Primary Scale of Intelligence-Third Edition (WPPSI-III; Wechsler, 2002). The raw scores on each subtest were transformed to age adjusted scaled scores. According to WPPSI-III standardization norms, each subset produces a scaled score with a mean of 10 and a standard deviation of 3. Scaled scores on the Block Design and the Matrix Reasoning subtests were first composited to create a non-verbal measure of IQ (r = .38, p b .01), which was then composited with the Vocabulary subscale scaled score (r = .49, p b .001) to yield a measure of IQ.
Parenting Quality (PQ) was scored by research assistants who completed five behavioral ratings immediately following a 90-minute structured interview with a parent, assessing how positively and warmly the parent spoke of the child, how hostile or rejecting the parent was in describing the child, closeness of parent and child, and the overall quality of the parent–child relationship (α = .89). These behavioral ratings were based on overall impressions of the parent's behavior and tone during the interview as well as specific reports of parenting practices, joint activities, displays of affections, school involvement, and many aspects of the child's behavior. In addition, the parent interviewer observed parent and child interactions during the short consent procedure. Parent interviewers were blind to children's performance on EC and IQ tests. Analogous rating scales showed high internal consistency, interrater agreement, and predictive validity in a previous study of homeless children (Milotis et al., 199
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.