About Pay for Performance in the Public Sector. I care about the Quality more the Quantity. Attached a review article about
Write me a paper of 4 to 8 pages. About Pay for Performance in the Public Sector.
I care about the Quality more the Quantity. Attached a review article about the same subject (Peer norm guesses and self-reported attitudes towards performance-related pay) Use this article to write the argument and if you agree or disagree.
- The First part will be about the authors and what they do (one paragraph).
- The Second part will be a summary and rephrase of the article’s conclusions (one Paragraph).
- The Third part will be the argument and if you agree or disagree with the authors point of view (three to four paragraphs).
If use any other resources please include a reference page, otherwise no need for it.
Thank you for your time.
also the paper is a MLA form but the professor wanted more informal
so it is not as a regular argument essay with Intro, and body and conclusion.
I wanted to be the same as I describe it in the discription.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Peer norm guesses and self-reported
attitudes towards performance-related pay
Nikolaos Georgantzis 1,2*, Efi Vasileiou2,3, Iordanis Kotzaivazoglou4
1 School of Agriculture Policy and Development, University of Reading, Reading, United Kingdom,
2 Laboratori d’Economia Experimental and Economics Department, Universitat Jaume I, Castellon, Spain,
3 University of Sheffield, International Faculty, City College, Thessaloniki, Greece, 4 Technological
Educational Institute of Central Macedonia, Serres, Greece
Abstract
Due to a variety of reasons, people see themselves differently from how they see others.
This basic asymmetry has broad consequences. It leads people to judge themselves and
their own behavior differently from how they judge others and others’ behavior. This
research, first, studies the perceptions and attitudes of Greek Public Sector employees
towards the introduction of Performance-Related Pay (PRP) systems trying to reveal
whether there is a divergence between individual attitudes and guesses on peers’ attitudes.
Secondly, it is investigated whether divergence between own self-reported and peer norm
guesses could mediate the acceptance of the aforementioned implementation once job sta-
tus has been controlled for. This study uses a unique questionnaire of 520 observations
which was designed to address the questions outlined in the preceding lines. Our economet-
ric results indicate that workers have heterogeneous attitudes and hold heterogeneous
beliefs on others’ expectations regarding a successful implementation of PRP. Specifically,
individual perceptions are less skeptical towards PRP than are beliefs on others’ attitudes.
Additionally, we found that managers are significantly more optimistic than lower rank
employees regarding the expected success of PRP systems in their jobs. However, they
both expect their peers to be more negative than they themselves are.
1. Introduction
It has been often documented that people tend to overestimate the extent to which their beliefs
coincide with a given social norm. Psychologists refer to this phenomenon with the term “false
consensus”.[1]. From a statistical point of view, it looks rather straightforward that, in the
absence of systematic biases, individual beliefs and individual perceptions of peer norms
should coincide at least on average. This is also compatible with a broadly accepted principle
according to which social norms play a central role in the formation of individual beliefs and
behavior [2] favoring the emergence of a (real) social consensus and “modal conformism” of
individual preferences. However, research in the field of social psychology has found that peo-
ple often misperceive the norms in their peer group. According to Epley and Dunning [3],
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174724 April 17, 2017 1 / 15
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Georgantzis N, Vasileiou E,
Kotzaivazoglou I (2017) Peer norm guesses and
self-reported attitudes towards performance-
related pay. PLoS ONE 12(4): e0174724. https://
doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174724
Editor: Pablo Brañas-Garza, Middlesex University, UNITED KINGDOM
Received: November 14, 2016
Accepted: March 14, 2017
Published: April 17, 2017
Copyright: © 2017 Georgantzis et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of
the Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: all relevant data are in
the paper and supporting information files
Funding: The authors received no specific funding
for this work
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist
such differences in how people see themselves versus others are rooted in basic processes of
human perception. It is almost axiomatic that as long as people are in a position to perceive
themselves and to perceive others, differences in those perceptions will exist and will engender
disagreement, misunderstanding, and conflict. Due to a variety of reasons, people see them-
selves differently from how they see others. They are immersed in their own sensations, emo-
tions, and cognitions at the same time that their experience of others is dominated by what can
be observed externally. This basic asymmetry has broad consequences. It leads people to judge
themselves and their own behavior differently from how they judge others and others’ behav-
ior [4]. Recent research in food choice [5] has revealed a systematic pattern of divergence
between individual preferences and individual beliefs regarding peer norms. In particular, it
has been observed that individuals tend to state a stronger (weaker) preference for healthy
(less healthy) food than they believe their peers do. The divergence vanishes for food which is
considered to be “neutral” (not particularly healthy or unhealthy). Therefore, individuals tend
to overestimate their acceptance of a socially desirable rule compared to what they would
guess regarding others. Humphrey [6] found that subordinate workers undervalue their fel-
lows’ subordinates and overvalue their managers. His findings are based on the structural cog-
nitive model assuming that organizational structures influence the information that actors
have about each other. The magnitude of the difference in how you perceive yourself from
how you perceive others is systematic and predictable. For example, “closer” others are per-
ceived in more self-like ways than more “distant” others [7]. However, the sign of the differ-
ence and its determinants are not fully understood.
Our research also relates to the literature on extrinsic versus intrinsic incentives in labor
relations. Following the literature on gift exchange, a reliable worker would reciprocate to a
generous employer in a way which does not require the performance-contingent, ex ante
establishment of compensating incentives rules like PR contracts [8]. In the Akerlof [8] model,
higher pay always leads to higher effort (though firms clearly would not wish to pay for
unbounded effort) and if worker effort depends on a reference wage, then it may be logical for
firms to pay a wage above that level to obtain extra effort. However, extrinsic incentives may
"crowd out" intrinsic ones, yielding the contrary effects to the pretended increased motivation
and higher effort [9, 10]. Behavioral economists recognized that extrinsic incentives (rewards)
may positively affect individuals only in the short run, but in the long run they might decrease
motivation [11]. They explain that extrinsic motivation has a limited impact on actual perfor-
mance while it may reduce the agent’s motivation to undertake similar tasks in the future. In
fact, [12] warn us that "there is no doubt that the benefits [piece-rate systems or pay-for-per-
formance incentive devices] can be considerably compromised when the systems undermine
workers’ intrinsic motivation. Some of these findings are confirmed by the perceived potential
effects of Performance-Related Pay (PRP) on intrinsic motivation. A variety of psychological
theories have been used to designate the role of pay in motivation. Nevertheless, expert opin-
ions for the effectiveness of pay as a motivator have historically been divided. Motivational the-
ories can be classified in two groups, according to the importance they assign to pay as a
motivator. The first group of theories, including Maslow’s need hierarchy theory [13], Herz-
berg’s motivation-hygiene theory [14] and Ryan and Deci’s [15] self-determination theory,
questions the contribution of monetary rewards to performance enhancement. It mainly
emphasizes internal motives and intrinsic rewards, such as recognition, social relations, self-
actualization, job enrichment or autonomy. It asserts that money helps employees to meet
only ‘basic needs’ and prevents dissatisfaction; however, pay cannot promote satisfaction.
Hence, the motivational effect of financial incentives may be not only limited, but also negative
[16]. On the contrary, the second group of theories, including reinforcement theory, expec-
tancy theory, equity theory or goal-setting theory, mainly concentrates on the process by
Attitudes towards performance-related pay: Me versus others
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174724 April 17, 2017 2 / 15
which desired behavior is stimulated and enhanced, and advocate that there is a strong, posi-
tive pay-motivation relationship [17]. In general terms, these theories argue that individuals
are motivated when they are challenged and reinforced to attain clear, measurable and de-
manding but reasonable goals with valued and fair rewards. They imply that for most employ-
ees, money is the primary reason for working; therefore, pay is potentially a very effective
means for improving performance [18].
We report results on the elicitation of beliefs and peer norm guesses providing evidence on
the divergence between individual attitudes and guesses on peers’ attitudes. We also find evi-
dence on the perceived negative effects of PRP on intrinsic motivation. Respondents report a
more optimistic expectation regarding the success of a PRP scheme in their working environ-
ment as compared to their guesses on their peers’ expectations. We find that managers are
more optimistic than clerical employees regarding the success of the scheme, but they both
expect their peers to be more negative than they themselves are. Overall, we believe that under-
standing the existence of these misperceptions may help us to understand perceived norms
and behavioral patterns in the working environment in order to successfully implement neces-
sary reforms in the public sector.
2. Data and measures
2.1. Respondents and procedure
The current study is based on data from public servants in northern Greece. The questionnaire
was administrated in January 2015 at the National Centre for Public Administration and Local
Government (EKDDA) and more specifically its decentralized Thessaloniki annex, PINEPTH.
PINEPTH covers the northern part of Greece and serves a population approaching 40% of the
public servants in Greece. The Thessaloniki Section of the Institute of Life-Long Education
(INEP) has among its objectives to educate those working in the public sector and the local
administration. The Institute belongs to the National Centre for Public Administration and
Local Government (EKDDA) which is the national strategic agent for the development of
Human Resources of the Public Administration and Local Government (http://www.ekdd.gr/
ekdda/index.php/en/2012-06-29-09-59-33). While attendance in the courses is voluntary, par-
ticipation of public employees is very high because of the weight the courses have in promo-
tions to a higher administrative rank. Therefore, access to the participants in the courses of the
institute guarantees a representative sample, including people from a broad range of geographi-
cal areas, hierarchy ranks and institutions. While the INEP evaluates participants in the frame-
work of the aforementioned courses and it may carry out its own research, the survey whose
data are used hereby was administered independently with a clear indication that the informa-
tion would be used under strict anonymity, exclusively for the purposes of this research, which
was undertaken by autonomous researchers belonging to academic institutions only.
A permission was first guaranteed by the INEP for the survey to be distributed to the atten-
dants in its courses. Further, it was made clear to them, that individual participation was vol-
untary. The Ethics Committee of the School of Agriculture Policy and Development at the
University of Reading approved this study.
We distributed a total of 840 questionnaires during the first break of the morning class.
Responses were collected at the end of the same day, allowing for about five hours within
which the survey could be completed. A relatively high rate of responses was obtained, with
584 questionnaires returned to us fully or partially completed. To avoid the problem of missing
variables, we used the sample of 520 complete responses.
The primary data for our assessment of PRP traits took the form of questionnaire responses
indicating personal perceptions of, and attitudes towards, general practice in the context of the
Attitudes towards performance-related pay: Me versus others
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174724 April 17, 2017 3 / 15
forthcoming adoption of PRP in the Greek public sector. It must be stressed that the definition
of PRP incentives and their precise characteristics are still subject to debate. As no standard
instrument for the assessment of PRP traits being available for this context, we designed our
own, as follow.
Each respondent was confronted with 11 questions, on each of which he/she was invited to
express an opinion on a 5-point scale. The scale ranged through strongly disagree (1), disagree
(2), unsure (3), agree (4), and strongly agree (5). Table 1 reports the definition of each variable.
The questions are numbered according to their appearance on the original questionnaire. Fre-
quency distributions of responses to each of the 11 attitudinal statements appear in Table 2.
The questionnaire additionally requested information on respondents´ on personal infor-
mation (such as gender) and on Job characteristics (position in service and job satisfaction).
The questionnaire included demographics and other individual information like years of
employment in the public sector, education, age, but have been omitted from the model, as
they did not appear to be significant in any of the specifications estimated.
2.2. Measures of “own perception” and “my perceptions on others’
perceptions”
As stated previously, people hold heterogeneous beliefs on others’ expectations. In order to
consider whether there is divergence between first- and second-order beliefs (my beliefs on
others’ beliefs). we use the following two questions allowing for “me versus others” compari-
sons. Therefore, the survey contained a question about employee’s own behavior (e.g. “In your opinion, could an introduction of a PRP system be effectively implemented in your job (sec- tion)?”), and a question about respondents’ perceptions of others’ attitudes (e.g. “In your opin- ion, which would your colleagues’ attitude be towards the introduction of a PRP system?”). Responses were provided on a scale from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree).
Fig 1 shows that approximately 20% of workers believe in a very successful implementation
of the PRP system in their division. Interestingly, the perception the workers have on their
coworkers’ reaction to the likelihood of a PRP system would be different. In terms of cowork-
ers’ expectations regarding PRP implementation only 8,3% of workers believe that others’ atti-
tude towards a PRP system could be positive. On the other hand, for those who believe that a
PRP system could be unsuccessfully implemented, 39,7% believe that this would arise due to
their coworkers’ reaction and only 27,9% due to their own.
Additionally, in Table 3, we present descriptive statistics of “own perception”, “my percep-
tion on others’ perceptions” and “me-vs-others misperceptions”. Misperceptions of others’
attitudes are calculated by subtracting the median of the behavior for each employee group
from each respondent’s reported perception of their employee’s behavior. Therefore, each
respondent has a misperception score which, if positive, indicated that they thought that other
employees would be more negative towards the implementation of a PRP system in their job
than their own attitudes. On average employees underestimate peers’ reaction on the success-
ful implementation of the PRP system with an own mean of 3.86 versus a mean of 3.17 for oth-
ers. To test whether there were significant misperceptions across the sample we conducted
one-sample t-tests comparing the mean misperception for each category with a value of zero.
All misperceptions were significantly different from zero.
3. Empirical methodology
This study followed the methodology for measuring self-vs.-other discrepancies between atti-
tudes by asking employees to express an opinion on what others would think regarding a possi- ble implementation of a PRP scheme in their job. The methodology employed is based on
Attitudes towards performance-related pay: Me versus others
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174724 April 17, 2017 4 / 15
regressions that relate the acceptability of PRP system to a set of attitudinal statements. The
definition variables included in the model are presented in Table 1. The dependent variable is
an ordered categorical variable taking on a finite number of outcomes, on a scale of 1 to 7, rep-
resenting the lowest and highest rating respectively. (“In your opinion, could an introduction of a PRP system be effectively implemented in your job (section)?”), which means that respondents
Table 1. Variables List.
Variables Definition
Attitudinal traits
Q 1. A PRP system help the workforce to improve
its productivity
Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Q. 2. A PRP system help public servants to better
understand the organization values and priorities
Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Q. 3.A PRP systems in public administration
discourage low-skilled applicants
Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Q. 4.A PRP system prompt employees to be
interested in tasks related to financial incentives
Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Q. 5.A PRP system lead a public servant to an
unethical behavior
Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Q. 6.A PRP systems demotivate public servants
that are intrinsically stimulated
Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Q. 7.A PRP system influence positively: supervisor-
employee relationship
Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Q. 8.A PRP system influence positively:
relationships with colleagues
Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Q. 9.A PRP system influence positively: total pay Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Q. 10.A PRP system influence positively: sense of
job security
Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Q. 11. A PRP system influence positively: tensions
in work
Dummy variable equal to 1 if the individuals; reported
the highest score in the five-point scale and 0
otherwise
Control variables
Male Dummy variable equal to 1 if the respondent is a male
Job satisfaction Standardized score of satisfaction with the job or
main activity where is measure on a seven-point scale
of 1 = totally dissatisfied to 7 = totally satisfied
Clerk Dummy variable equal to 1 if the respondent has a
clerical position
Dependent Variables
Own perception of the effective implementation of a
PRP system
Standardized score of an individuals’ own perception
where is measure on a seven-point scale of 1 = totally
disagree to 7 = totally agree
Own perception of others’ perceptions of the
effective implementation of a PRP system
Standardized score of an individuals’ own perception
of others ‘perceptions where is measure on a seven-
point scale of 1 = totally disagree to 7 = totally agree
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174724.t001
Attitudes towards performance-related pay: Me versus others
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174724 April 17, 2017 5 / 15
can only express their responses on a non-linear scale. Therefore, the researcher does not
know the respondent’s exact feeling but only the interval to which she/he belongs and for this
reason, the respondent’s attitude is assumed to be a latent variable that is not directly observ-
able and thus, an ordered probit model is applied.
In an ordered probit model, the latent probability of reporting a level of successfully imple-
mentation of a PRP system P �
is: Own perception of the effective implementation of a PRP sys- tem: Pi = F
−1 (Pi
�
) = ϑXi + δSi + εi eijX * Normal(0,1) (Model 1), where Pi is the own perceived implementation of the PRP system in the public sector stated by individual i. Xi are the 11 atti- tudinal variables. Si is a vector of control variables and εi is the error term. Assuming that μ1 < μ2 < . . . < μj where μ1,. . .μj are the cutoff points for the latent variable
P� ¼
0; if P�� m 1
1; if m 1 � P�� m
2
. . .
j; if P� > mj
8 >>>>><
>>>>>:
the parameters θ,δ and μ can be estimated by maximum likelihood.
Table 2. Questionnaire responses, Descriptive statistics.
Questions Responses (%)
Independent variables—Attitudinal traits Scale 1 Scale 2 Scale 3 Scale 4 Scale 5
Q. 1. In your opinion, to what extend does a PRP system in public service help the
workforce to improve its productivity?
7.4 9.5 16.6 47.8 18.7
Q. 2. In your opinion, to what extent does a PRP system help public servants to better
understand the organization values and priorities?
11.7 18 19.4 41.7 9.2
Q. 3. In your opinion, to what extent do PRP systems in public administration
discourage low-skilled employees (applicants)?
18.8 21.5 22.7 27.3 9.7
Q. 4. In your opinion, to what extent do PRP systems prompt public servants to be
interested solely in tasks that are directly related to financial incentives?
2.7 6.5 20 51.3 19.5
Q. 5. In your opinion, to what extent may a PRP system lead a public servant to an
unethical behavior?
12.6 20.4 24.6 32.2 10.2
Q. 6. In your opinion, to what extent may PRP systems demotivate public servants that
are intrinsically stimulated?
21 21.9 25.2 24.4 7.5
Scale from 1 (negative) to 5 (positive) 1 2 3 4 5
Q. 7. In your opinion, in which way could an introduction of a PRP system influence:
supervisor-employee relationship
12.8 20.2 34.7 21.9 10.4
Q. 8. In your opinion, in which way could an introduction of a PRP system influence:
relationships with colleagues
21.5 31.3 31.5 11.3 4.4
Q. 9. In your opinion, in which way could an introduction of a PRP system influence:
total pay
3.6 6.2 22.4 41.2 26.6
Q. 10 In your opinion, in which way could an introduction of a PRP system influence:
sense of job security
17.8 19.8 33.3 20.6 8.5
Q. 11. In your opinion, in which way could an introduction of a PRP system influence:
tensions in work
16 19.4 23.3 26.8 14.5
Your Job satisfaction from 1 to 7 11.1 13 22.5 31.4 22
Dependent Variables Strongly disagree
(Scale 1–2)
DisagreeScale
3
UnsureScale
4
Agree
Scale 5
Strongly
agree
(Scale 6–7)
In your opinion, could an introduction of a PRP system be effectively implemented in
your job (section)?
27.9 13.5 16.8 21.9 19.9
In your opinion, how could be your colleagues ‘reaction to the introduction of a PRP
system?
39.8 19.1 18.9 13.8 8.4
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174724.t002
Attitudes towards performance-related pay: Me versus others
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174724 April 17, 2017 6 / 15
In our model, we transform the five-point scale of the 11 attitudinal variables to binary vari-
ables. Therefore, we create 11 dummy variables equal to 1 if the individuals reported the high-
est score in the five-point scale and 0 otherwise.
The regression was re-run for the coworkers’ perception to identify differences in the
explanatory factors of own and others’ perceptions using an ordered probit model. The depen-
dent variable is again an ordered categorical variable based on the following question “In your opinion, which would your colleagues’ attitude be towards the introduction of a PRP system?” range from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree).
A second model is specified to study Individual perceptions on others beliefs of the effective implementation of a PRP system: Li = αXi + λSi + ui (Model 2), where Li is individual i perception over the others’ perceptions for the successful implementation of the PRP system in the public
sector. Xi are the 11 attitudinal variables. Si is a vector of control variables and ui is the error term. The parameters α,λ and μ (the cutoff points for the latent variable) will be estimated by maximum likelihood. The results of the regressions are presented in Tables 4, 5 and 6.
4. Results
This section presents: a) the results of the employee’ perception about the successful imple-
mentation of a PRP system in the public sector (Table 4, column 1), b) the estimation results
Fig 1. Own attitudes and beliefs regarding peers’ attitude towards PRP.
https://doi.org/10.1371
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.