Summarize the book chapter in your own words (3-6 paragraphs). Make sure you explain what the theory is and how people are affected. Explain why this theory
Summarize the book chapter in your own words (3-6 paragraphs). Make sure you explain what the theory is and how people are affected.
Explain why this theory is important (one paragraph).
Discuss one current media example related to the theory. It can be a scene from a movie, TV show, videogame, Facebook, etc. Make sure you explain how the example illustrates the theory. (2-3 paragraphs) Please include a link to the content if possible (it is okay if no link is available).
Explain what action, if any, you would take if you were in charge of the example you discussed (refuse to show it, edit the content, add warnings, include additional content, etc.). Make sure you explain why you would take that action. (1-2 paragraphs)
Discuss how you think this media effect has affected you personally (either you were affected or someone you know was affected). Was the effect strong or weak? What would have made the effect stronger or weaker? (2-3 paragraphs)
1
HOW THE NEWS SHAPES OUR CIVIC AGENDA
Maxwell McCombs University of Texas at Austin
Amy Reynolds Indiana University at Bloomington
The war in Iraq dwarfed all other topics reported in the U.S. news media during the first three months of 2007. And public opinion polls during the same time showed that Americans thought the Iraq War was the most important issue as they began to think about electing a new president in 2008. Through their day-to-day selection and display of the news, journalists focus our attention and influence our perceptions of the most important issues facing the country, and in early 2007 the focus was on the Iraq war. This ability to influence the salience of topics on the public agenda has come to be called the agenda-setting role of the news media.
Establishing this salience among the public so that an issue becomes the focus of public attention, thought, and perhaps even action is the initial stage in the formation of public opinion. While many issues compete for public attention, only a few are success- ful in capturing public attention. The news media exert significant influence on our perceptions of what are the most salient issues of the day.
Because people use the media to help them sort through important political issues before they vote, scholars have spent nearly 70 years studying the effect of mass com- munication on voters. In the 1940 U.S. presidential election, Paul Lazarsfeld and his colleagues at Columbia University collaborated with pollster Elmo Roper to conduct seven rounds of interviews with voters in Erie County, Ohio (Lazarsfeld, Berelson, & Gaudet, 1944). Those surveys and many subsequent investigations in other settings over the next 20 years found little evidence of major mass communication effects on atti- tudes and opinions. Many scholars have argued that little evidence of effects was found because these early studies focused on the mass media’s ability to persuade voters and change their attitudes. However, traditional journalism norms emphasize that the media are trying to inform, not persuade. These early studies did support that idea, demon- strating that people acquired information from the mass media, even if they didn’t change their opinions. In an ironic turn of history for communication research, recent elabor- ations of agenda-setting theory discussed later in this chapter are investigating the relationship between the effects of agenda setting and public opinions and attitudes (Kim & McCombs, 2007).
But, as a result of the early election studies, a limited-effects model for mass
1 Media Effects : Advances in Theory and Research, edited by Mary Beth Oliver, Taylor & Francis, 2009. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/pensu/detail.action?docID=380841. Created from pensu on 2017-07-30 11:26:29.
C op
yr ig
ht ©
2 00
9. T
ay lo
r &
F ra
nc is
. A ll
rig ht
s re
se rv
ed .
communication emerged. Summarized in the law of minimal consequences (Klapper, 1960), this notion ran counter to the ideas that Walter Lippmann, the intellectual father of agenda setting, proposed back in the early 1920s. Lippmann’s opening chapter in his 1922 classic, Public Opinion, which is titled “The World Outside and the Pictures in Our Heads,” summarizes the agenda-setting idea even though he did not use that phrase. His thesis is that the news media, our windows to the vast world beyond our direct expe- rience, determine our cognitive maps of that world. Public opinion, argued Lippmann, responds not to the environment, but to the pseudo-environment, the world constructed by the news media (Lippmann, 1922).
This scientific shift of perspective away from the law of minimal consequences took hold in the 1960s, and during the 1968 presidential election McCombs and Shaw (1972) launched a seminal study that would support Lippmann’s notion that the information provided by the news media plays a key role in the construction of our pictures of reality. Their central hypothesis was that the mass media set the agenda of issues for a political campaign by influencing the salience of issues among voters. Those issues emphasized in the news come to be regarded over time as important by members of the public. McCombs and Shaw called this hypothesized influence agenda setting.
To test this hypothesis that the media agenda can set the public agenda, McCombs and Shaw conducted a survey among a sample of randomly selected undecided voters in Chapel Hill, North Carolina. These undecided voters were asked to name what they thought were the key issues of the day, regardless of what the candidates were saying. The issues named in the survey were ranked according to the percentage of voters naming each one to yield a description of the public agenda.
Concurrent with this survey of voters, the nine major news sources used by these voters—five local and national newspapers, two television networks, and two news magazines—were collected and content analyzed. The rank-order of issues on the media agenda was determined by the number of news stories devoted to each issue. The high degree of correspondence between these two agendas of political and social issues established a central link in what has become a substantial chain of evidence for an agenda-setting role of the press.
If this correlation between the voters’ agenda and the total news agenda was the highest, it would be evidence of agenda setting. If the correlation with the voters’ pre- ferred party’s agenda in the news coverage was higher, it would be evidence of selective perception. The concept of selective perception, which is often cited as an explanation for minimal media effects, locates central influence within the individual and stratifies media content according to its compatibility with an individual’s existing attitudes and opinions. From this perspective, it is assumed that individuals minimize their exposure to non-supportive information and maximize their exposure to supportive information. The vast majority of the Chapel Hill evidence favored an agenda-setting effect.
ACCUMULATED EVIDENCE
Since the Chapel Hill study, researchers have conducted more than 425 empirical studies on the agenda-setting influence of the news media. This vast accumulated evidence comes from many different geographic and historical settings worldwide and covers numerous types of news media and a wide variety of public issues. The evidence also provides greater detail about the time-order and causal links between the media and public agendas.
2
M A X W E L L M c C O M B S A N D A M Y R E Y N O L D S
Media Effects : Advances in Theory and Research, edited by Mary Beth Oliver, Taylor & Francis, 2009. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/pensu/detail.action?docID=380841. Created from pensu on 2017-07-30 11:26:29.
C op
yr ig
ht ©
2 00
9. T
ay lo
r &
F ra
nc is
. A ll
rig ht
s re
se rv
ed .
Shaw and McCombs’ (1977) follow-up to the Chapel Hill study examined a represen- tative sample of all voters in Charlotte, North Carolina during the summer and fall of the 1972 presidential election and found that the salience of all seven issues on the public agenda was influenced by the pattern of news coverage in the Charlotte Observer and network television news. During the 1976 presidential election, voters in three very different settings—Lebanon, New Hampshire; Indianapolis, Indiana; and Evanston, Illinois—were interviewed nine times between February and December (Weaver, Graber, McCombs, & Eyal, 1981). Simultaneously, election coverage by the three national networks and local newspapers in the three cities was content analyzed. In all three communities the agenda-setting influence of both television and newspapers was greatest during the spring primaries.
Although election settings provide a strong natural laboratory in which to study agenda-setting effects, the evidence that supports the theory is not limited to elections. Winter and Eyal (1981) took a historical look at the civil rights issue between 1954 and 1976 using 27 Gallup polls. Comparison of the trends in public opinion with a content analysis of the New York Times yielded a correlation of +.71. Similar findings about the impact of news coverage on trends in public opinion come from an analysis of 11 different issues during a 41-month period in the 1980s (Eaton, 1989). In each of these analyses, the media agenda is based on a mix of television, newspapers, and news maga- zines, while the public agenda is based on 13 Gallup polls. All but one of the correla- tions (the issue of morality) were positive, although a pattern of considerable variability in the strength of the correlations was visible.
More recently, Holbrook and Hill (2005) explored the agenda-setting effect of enter- tainment media. They used data from two controlled lab experiments and the 1995 National Election Study Pilot Study to show that the viewing of crime dramas signifi- cantly increased concerns about crime and that those concerns affected viewers’ opinions of the president. Gross and Aday (2003) compared the effects of watching local televi- sion news with direct experience measures of crime on fear of victimization and issue salience. They found that local news exposure did account for an agenda-setting effect. They did not, however, find that television viewing cultivated fear of becoming a victim of crime. Hester and Gibson (2003) combined data from four years of print and broad- cast news about the economy in time series analyses with two indicators of consumer economic evaluations and three measures of real economic conditions. They concluded that the media’s emphasis on negative economic news may have serious consequences for both economic performance and expectations.
Agenda-setting effects also have also been found outside of the U.S. In Pamplona, Spain during the spring of 1995, comparisons of six major concerns on the public agenda with local news coverage showed a high degree of correspondence (Canel, Llamas, & Rey, 1996). In Germany, a look at national public opinion patterns during 1986 through weekly comparisons of the public and media agendas showed that television news coverage had a significant impact on public concern about five issues, including the country’s energy supply (Brosius & Kepplinger, 1990). Early in 1986 the energy supply issue had little salience on either the news agenda or the public agenda. But a rapid rise in May on the news agenda was followed within a week by a similar rise on the public agenda. When news coverage subsequently declined, so did the size of the constituency expressing concern about Germany’s energy supply.
Again at the local level, agenda setting occurred in the October 1997 legislative elec- tions in the Buenos Aires metropolitan area (Lennon, 1998). In September, the public agenda and the combined issue agenda of five major Buenos Aires newspapers only
3
C I V I C AG E N DA
Media Effects : Advances in Theory and Research, edited by Mary Beth Oliver, Taylor & Francis, 2009. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/pensu/detail.action?docID=380841. Created from pensu on 2017-07-30 11:26:29.
C op
yr ig
ht ©
2 00
9. T
ay lo
r &
F ra
nc is
. A ll
rig ht
s re
se rv
ed .
modestly agreed overall, but as election day approached in October, the correspondence between the agendas soared, an increase that suggests considerable learning from the news media in the closing weeks of the election campaign.
Using a cross-national comparative perspective that involved 14 European Union (EU) member states, Peter (2003) explored whether the amount of television coverage of the EU affected the extent to which EU citizens perceived European integration to be important. He found that the more EU stories people watched in countries in which the political elites disagreed about integration the more important they considered integration. But in countries in which elite opinion about integration was consensual this pattern did not repeat.
These real-world examples of agenda-setting effects are compelling but are not the best evidence for the core, causal proposition of agenda setting. The best evidence that the news media are the cause of these kinds of effects comes from controlled laboratory experiments, a setting where the theorized cause can be systematically manip- ulated, subjects are randomly assigned to various versions of the manipulation, and systematic comparisons are made among the outcomes. Evidence from laboratory experiments provides the final link in agenda setting’s causal chain.
Changes in the salience of defense preparedness, pollution, arms control, civil rights, unemployment, and a number of other issues were produced in the laboratory among subjects who viewed TV news programs edited to emphasize a particular issue (Iyengar & Kinder, 1987). A variety of controls were used to show that changes in the salience of the manipulated issue were actually due to exposure to the news agenda. For example, in one experiment, control subjects viewed TV news programs that did not include the issue of defense preparedness. The change in salience of this issue was significantly higher for the test subjects who viewed stories on defense preparedness than for the subjects in the control group. There were no significant differences between the two groups from before to after viewing the newscasts for seven other issues. And a recent experiment docu- mented the agenda-setting effects of an online newspaper. The salience of racism as a public issue was significantly higher among all three groups of subjects exposed to vari- ous versions of an online newspaper that discussed racism than among those subjects whose online newspaper did not contain a news report on racism (Wang, 2000).
These studies are far from all of the accumulated evidence that supports the theory of agenda setting. A meta-analysis of 90 empirical agenda-setting studies found a mean correlation of +.53, with most about six points above or below the mean (Wanta & Ghanem, 2000). There are, of course, a number of significant influences that shape individual attitudes and public opinion, including a person’s personal experience as well as their exposure to the mass media. But the general proposition supported by this accumulation of evidence on agenda setting is that journalists do significantly influence their audience’s picture of the world.
Many events and stories compete for journalists’ attention. Because journalists have neither the capacity to gather all information nor the capacity to inform the audience about every single occurrence, they rely on a traditional set of professional norms to guide their daily sampling of the environment. The result is a limited view of the larger environment, something like the highly limited view of the outside world available through a small window.
Four portraits of public opinion—the major issues of the 1960s, the drug issue in the 1980s, crime in the 1990s, and the economy in the 2000s—tell us a great deal about the discretion of journalists and the discrepancies that are sometimes found in mass media portrayals of reality. In Funkhouser’s (1973) study of public opinion trends
4
M A X W E L L M c C O M B S A N D A M Y R E Y N O L D S
Media Effects : Advances in Theory and Research, edited by Mary Beth Oliver, Taylor & Francis, 2009. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/pensu/detail.action?docID=380841. Created from pensu on 2017-07-30 11:26:29.
C op
yr ig
ht ©
2 00
9. T
ay lo
r &
F ra
nc is
. A ll
rig ht
s re
se rv
ed .
during the 1960s, there was no correlation at all between the trends in news coverage of major issues and the reality of these issues. But there was a substantial correlation (+.78) between the patterns of news coverage and the public’s perception of what were the most important issues. In the 1980s, there was an increasing trend in news coverage of drugs at a time when there was no change at all in the reality of the drug problem (Reese & Danielian, 1989). In the 1990s, there was an increase in the news coverage of crime at a time when there was a decreasing trend in the reality of crime (Ghanem, 1996). And, at the turn of the 21st century, Hester and Gibson (2003) noted that media coverage of the economy may have serious consequences for economic expectations and performance, particularly when the coverage is negative.
THE ACAPULCO TYPOLOGY
Explorations of agenda-setting effects have observed this mass communication phe- nomenon from a variety of perspectives. A four-part typology describing these perspec- tives is frequently referred to as the Acapulco typology because McCombs initially presented it in Acapulco, Mexico at the invitation of International Communication Association president Everett Rogers. The Acapulco typology contains two dichotom- ous dimensions. The first dimension distinguishes between two ways of looking at agendas. The focus of attention can be on the entire set of items that define the agenda, or the focus of attention can be narrowed to a single, particular item on the agenda. The second dimension distinguishes between two ways of measuring the salience of items on the agenda, either aggregate measures describing an entire population or measures that describe individual responses.
Perspective I includes the entire agenda and uses aggregate measures of the popula- tion to establish the salience of these items. The original Chapel Hill study took this perspective. For the media agenda, the salience of the issues was determined by the total percentage of news articles on each issue, while the public agenda was determined by the percentage of voters who thought the government should do something about each issue. This perspective is named “competition” because it examines an array of issues competing for positions on the agenda.
Perspective II is similar to the early agenda-setting studies with their focus on the entire agenda of items, but it shifts the focus to the agenda of each individual. When individuals are asked to rank-order a series of issues, there is little evidence of any correspondence at all between those individual rankings and the rank-order of those same issues in the news media. This perspective is labeled “automaton” because of its unflattering view of human behavior. An individual seldom reproduces to any significant degree the entire agenda of the media.
Perspective III narrows the focus to a single item on the agenda but like perspective I uses aggregate measures to establish salience. Commonly, the measures are the total number of news stories about the item and the percentage of the public citing an issue as the most important problem facing the country. This perspective is named “natural history” because the focus typically is on the degree of correspondence between the media agenda and the public agenda in the rise and fall of a single item over time. An example of this perspective is Winter and Eyal’s (1981) study of the issue of civil rights over a 23-year period.
Perspective IV, like perspective II, focuses on the individual, but it narrows its observations to the salience of a single agenda item. This perspective, named “cognitive
5
C I V I C AG E N DA
Media Effects : Advances in Theory and Research, edited by Mary Beth Oliver, Taylor & Francis, 2009. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/pensu/detail.action?docID=380841. Created from pensu on 2017-07-30 11:26:29.
C op
yr ig
ht ©
2 00
9. T
ay lo
r &
F ra
nc is
. A ll
rig ht
s re
se rv
ed .
portrait,” is illustrated by the experimental studies of agenda setting in which the salience of a single issue for an individual is measured before and after exposure to news programs where the amount of exposure to various issues is controlled.
The existence of these varied perspectives on the agenda-setting phenomenon, espe- cially an abundance of evidence based on perspectives I and III, strengthens the degree of confidence about this media effect. Perspective I provides useful, comprehensive descriptions of the rich, ever-changing mix of mass media content and public opinion at particular points in time. This perspective strives to describe the world as it is. Perspective III provides useful descriptions of the natural history of a single issue but at the expense of the larger social context. Despite this, knowledge about the dynamics of a single issue over an extended time period is useful for understanding how the process of agenda setting works. Perspective IV also makes a valuable contribution to understanding the dynamics of agenda setting. From a scholarly viewpoint, evidence generated by perspectives III and IV is absolutely necessary for a detailed “how” and “why” explanation of agenda setting. But the ultimate goal of agenda-setting theory returns us to perspective I, which provides a comprehensive view of mass communication and public opinion in communities and nations.
ATTRIBUTE AGENDA SETTING
In most discussions of the agenda-setting role of the mass media, the unit of analysis on each agenda is an object, usually a public issue. But public issues are not the only objects that can be analyzed from the agenda-setting perspective. In party primaries, the objects of interest are the candidates vying for the nomination of their political party. Communication is a process that can be about any object or set of objects competing for attention. In all these instances, the term object is used in the same sense that social psychologists use the term attitude object.
Beyond the agenda of objects, there is another level of agenda setting. Each of the objects on an agenda has numerous attributes—characteristics and properties that describe the object. Just as objects vary in salience, so do their attributes. Both the selection of objects for attention and the selection of attributes for picturing those objects are powerful agenda-setting roles. An important part of the news agenda is the attributes that journalists and, subsequently, members of the public have in mind when they think about and talk about each object. These attributes have two dimen- sions, a cognitive component regarding information about substantive characteristics that describe the object and an affective component regarding the positive, negative, or neutral tone of these characteristics on the media agenda or the public agenda. The influence of attribute agendas in the news on the public’s attribute agenda is the second level of agenda setting.
In an election setting, the theoretical distinction between the agenda of objects (the candidates) and the agendas of attributes (their images) is especially clear. Voters’ images of the Democrat candidates during the 1976 presidential primaries illustrate this second level of agenda-setting effects. Eleven candidates were vying to be the Democrat challenger to incumbent Republican president Gerald Ford. Comparisons of New York voters’ descriptions of these candidates with Newsweek’s attribute agenda in its candidate sketches showed significant evidence of media influence (Becker & McCombs, 1978). Similar media effects on voters’ images of political candidates have been found in such diverse cultural settings as the 1996 Spanish general election
6
M A X W E L L M c C O M B S A N D A M Y R E Y N O L D S
Media Effects : Advances in Theory and Research, edited by Mary Beth Oliver, Taylor & Francis, 2009. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/pensu/detail.action?docID=380841. Created from pensu on 2017-07-30 11:26:29.
C op
yr ig
ht ©
2 00
9. T
ay lo
r &
F ra
nc is
. A ll
rig ht
s re
se rv
ed .
(McCombs, Lopez-Escobar, & Llamas, 2000), the 1994 mayoral election in Taipei, Taiwan (King, 1997), and the 2002 Texas elections for governor and U.S. senator (Kim & McCombs, 2007).
Salience, which has been a central focus of agenda-setting theory, also can be exam- ined at a second level. Public issues, like all other objects, have attributes. Different aspects of issues—their attributes—are emphasized to varying degrees in the news and in how people think and talk about issues. Again demonstrating the validity of agenda-setting theory across cultures, analysis of the 1993 Japanese general election found effects at both the first and second levels for the issue of political reform (Takeshita & Mikami, 1995). The more people used the news media, the greater the overall salience of the issue of political reform and, in particular, the greater the salience of system-related aspects of political reform, the aspect of the issue emphasized in the news.
Beyond election settings, in Minneapolis the correspondence between the local newspaper’s reporting on the state of the economy and the salience of specific eco- nomic problems, causes, and proposed solutions among the public was a robust +.81 (Benton & Frazier, 1976). For an environmental issue in Indiana, the degree of cor- respondence was +.71 between the local newspaper’s coverage of various aspects of this issue and the public’s perspective on the development of a large lake (Cohen, 1975). In Japan, the correspondence between the coverage in two major dailies of the aspects of global environmental problems and Tokyo residents’ concerns about these prob- lems reached a peak of +.78 just prior to the United Nations’ 1992 Rio de Janeiro environmental conference (Mikami, Takeshita, Nakada, & Kawabata, 1994).
Explication of attribute agenda setting also links the theory with the concept of framing. Both framing and attribute agenda setting call attention to the perspectives used by communicators and their audiences to picture topics in the daily news. How- ever, because of the large number of definitions for framing, comparisons of the two approaches range from substantial overlap to total dissimilarity. Recent research has identified two types of frames, aspects and central themes, that do greatly resemble attribute agendas (McCombs, 2004).
An example of the aspects perspective, Ashley and Olson’s (1998) catalog of the frames in news coverage of the women’s movement ranges from feminists’ appearance, used in more than a fourth of stories, to the seldom cited goals of the movement. Illustrating the convergence of the aspects framing perspective with attribute agenda setting, Miller, Andsager, and Riechert (1998) identified 28 frames describing four major candidates seeking the 1996 Republican presidential nomination. Although the study focused exclusively on identification of the frames in the campaign press releases and in news stories, subsequent analysis documented substantial attribute agenda-setting effects of the press releases on the news stories (McCombs, 2004).
In other framing research, the focus is on the dominant attribute defining the central theme of the news stories. Significant differences in the audience’s responses were found in Nelson, Clawson, and Oxley’s (1997) experiment comparing the effects of two contrasting themes, free speech versus public order, in news stories about a KKK rally and in McLeod and Detenber’s (1999) experiment with news stories whose central theme varied in their level of support for civil protest. The idea that certain attributes of an object are compelling arguments for their salience (Ghanem, 1996) further links framing and agenda setting. Ghanem found that crime stories with low psychological distance—the crimes occurred locally or easily could happen to the audi- ence member—drove the salience of crime as a public issue in Texas. Sheafer (2007) found that the negative valence of news stories was a compelling argument for the
7
C I V I C AG E N DA
Media Effects : Advances in Theory and Research, edited by Mary Beth Oliver, Taylor & Francis, 2009. ProQuest Ebook Central, http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/pensu/detail.action?docID=380841. Created from pensu on 2017-07-30 11:26:29.
C op
yr ig
ht ©
2 00
9. T
ay lo
r &
F ra
nc is
. A ll
rig ht
s re
se rv
ed .
salience of the economy as the most important problem during
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.
