Submit a reflection on the Myths and Facts activity. In your reflection: ? Identify at least one myth that you had previously believed to be true. Is it
https://cdn-media.waldenu.edu/2dett4d/Walden/SOCW/6103/MF/index.html#/
Submit a 2-**** reflection on the Myths and Facts activity. In your reflection:
- Identify at least one myth that you had previously believed to be true. Is it challenging for you to now accept the true information, and why?
- Describe the information on which you based your answers (e.g., prior knowledge, past experiences, media portrayals).
- Explain how you plan on ensuring your information is accurate as a social worker.
Van Wormer, K., & Davis, D. R. (2018). Addiction treatment: A strengths perspective (4th ed.). Cengage Learning.
- Chapter 3, “Substance Misuse, Dependence, and the Body” (pp. 91–149)
EDITORIAL Open Access
Preventing sensationalistic science and fake news about substance use Stephan Arndt1,2,3* and De Shauna Jones1
An accurately informed public is vital to creating ra- tional substance use, prevention, and treatment policies. Unfortunately, the general public neither has the train- ing nor the time to conduct intensive investigations. Most people get their information via the news media, newsletters, and as sound bites. People often accept what they read as true, especially if it comes from a rea- sonably reputable source and do not question the infor- mation however astounding or alarming. It is easy to forget the adage, “Extraordinary claims require extraor- dinary evidence.” To make matters worse, people may tend to repeat the more remarkable information, regard- less of how accurate it is. I recently investigated two remarkable pieces of infor-
mation regarding substance use issues. These both per- tained to opioids, but might be equally enlightening no matter what substance was involved.
Case 1 I recently read that 14–22% of all pregnancies in the United States were complicated by use of prescription opioid medications. This was in a newsletter [1] referen- cing a paper in the BMJ [2]. The newsletter description goes on to say that an infant showing signs of opioid withdrawal is born every 25 min. Despite their proxim- ity, the two statistics represent different things. A reader might easily take this statement to suggest
that about one in five births are complicated by opioid use. We were reasonably surprised by that and so read the original article. Interestingly, results of the refer- enced paper by Huybrechts et al. [2] indicated no such thing. Instead, the statement in the newsletter referen- cing 14 to 22% was in Huybrechts et al’s [2] introduction and was attributed to two previous papers (reference numbers 3 and 4). One of the first source papers
suggested that 14.4% of women were prescribed opioid medication at some point in their pregnancy [3] while the other reference found that 22% of Medicaid women were prescribed opioids at some point in their pregnancy [4]. Note that these first source references suggest the 9- month prevalence of receiving an opioid medication dur- ing pregnancy and do not propose that the resulting birth suffered opioid withdrawal or that there were any complications. The article, referenced by the newsletter [2], found
that only 2.3% of births from women receiving opioid medication during their last trimester suffered discern- able withdrawal. Therefore, the actual percentage of births suffering from withdrawal would be 0.023 times either 0.14 or 0.22 depending on whether we used the first sources’ estimates of 14.4% or 22%. Either of these calculations result in an estimate of less than 1% (0.3312% or 0.506%) and not 14 to 22%. Given that the periods of prescription were different for the source pa- pers (9 months) and the article reference by the newslet- ter (3 months), the prevalence of births complicated by opioids is likely lower. The statement that the one infant is born every
25 min came from another study [5] suggesting that neonatal abstinence syndrome rose from 0.34% to 0.58% between 2009 to 2012. This is similar to the estimate in the previous paragraph, 0.3312% or 0.506%, but a far cry from the estimates in the newsletter [1] and its refer- enced paper’s introduction [2], "14-22% of pregnancies are complicated due to the use of prescription opioid medications". The only way this can be interpreted as a correct statement is to define any opioid medication taken at any time during pregnancy as a complication. If the initially referenced authors and newsletter were equating any opioid prescription as a complication, then the equivalence was not transparent.
Case 2 This is more of a generic situation rather than a specific instance. While speaking at a local school about opioid issues in Iowa, a teacher asked me "How many pain pills
* Correspondence: [email protected] 1Iowa Consortium for Substance Abuse Research and Evaluation, University of Iowa, 100 MTP4, Iowa City, IA 52245-5000, USA 2Department of Psychiatry, Carver College of Medicine, University of Iowa, 200 Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA Full list of author information is available at the end of the article
© The Author(s). 2018 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Arndt and Jones Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy (2018) 13:11 https://doi.org/10.1186/s13011-018-0148-3
did it take to cause irreparable brain damage? Is it one, two, four?". While it is certainly a possibility that an opioid over-
dose might cause oxygen deprivation, which in turn could cause brain damage, I do not think that was what the teacher was asking. He was asking about the direct damage caused by an opioid. Clearly, people take opioids for pain management from injury and surgery or cancer pain. Sometimes, the course of opioid treatment is days, even weeks. There is no evidence for brain damage in these situations. I did find some threads in discussion boards that claimed that certain spices (e.g., turmeric) could reverse the brain damage from opioid use, but these were unsupported “junk science” comments. The magazine, Scientific American, had an online art-
icle "Can HGH Reverse Brain Damage in Drug Addicts?" [6] This article starts out in the first paragraph with the statement that opiates destroy brain cells, with no sup- porting peer reviewed references. The article goes on to describe a study in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, which shows that mouse neuronal cells in a petri dish bathed in morphine tend to die off. However, when also exposed to synthetic growth hor- mone, the die off was attenuated [7]. Several studies sug- gest little or no effects of prescribed opioid use and cognition [8–10]. While there may be some evidence that opioids cause apoptosis in neonates [11] and that extensive use of illicit opioids are associated with cogni- tive issues, there is scant evidence that they cause cell death or “brain damage” in adults. These references do not address whether or not opioids cause anatomical brain damage. The long term and widespread use of opi- oids for surgeries might suggest otherwise. Thus, the wording in the title, although catchy, may be misleading. There are several things authors, reviewers, and edi-
tors can do to prevent misunderstandings and fake news. The simplest is to watch for odd and surprising statistics presented anywhere in an article. Remarkable comments need scrutiny. Papers need to highlight the importance of their work, but they also need to remain trustworthy and should not exaggerate. Hidden presumptions should not be a part of scientific literature since scientific com- munication demands transparent and replicable evidence. Manuscripts require precise and accurate language de-
void of hidden agendas. Words such as “cognitive im- pairment”, “deficiency”, and “deficits” appear often in scientific literature with regard to opiates and medica- tion assisted treatment (MAT) effects. Of course, these papers are seldom experimental designs that can assess causal effects. Furthermore, these terms sound far more “ominous” than noting that the people in the opioid use group or MAT group had lower performance than the comparison group. For example, the differences between
groups on some measure of cognitive functioning should be called a “difference”. Consider two groups where Group A has lower scores than Group B. If Group A were opioid users in treatment and Group B were a community sample who were age and sex matched, an author might suggest that Group A had cognitive “defi- cits”. However, hidden assumptions suddenly show up if instead of selecting Group A based on their opioid use we selected these people based their skin color or ethnic background. The appropriate neutral word would be “difference”. Stigma, fear, and exaggeration are poor motivators to
improve science or policy relating to substance use is- sues. In fact, we as scientists and educators should be represent reasoned rationality rather than sensational- ism. Exaggeration, hidden presumptions that misinform, and unstated agenda are the antithesis of scientific litera- ture and represents “fake news” and “junk science”. Au- thors need to present facts clearly and transparently. Reviewers and editors, as the guardians of rational and transparent information, need to question authors’ about wording issues, presumptions, and remarkable statements.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Author details 1Iowa Consortium for Substance Abuse Research and Evaluation, University of Iowa, 100 MTP4, Iowa City, IA 52245-5000, USA. 2Department of Psychiatry, Carver College of Medicine, University of Iowa, 200 Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA. 3Department of Biostatistics, College of Public Health, University of Iowa, 145 N. Riverside Drive, Iowa City, IA 52245-5000, USA.
Received: 1 March 2018 Accepted: 6 March 2018
References 1. Addiction Technology Transfer Center (ATTC) Network. Families In Focus
Newsletter, September 2017. Families In Focus. 2017;2: http://attcppwtools. org/Newsletters/2017-09.aspx.
2. Huybrechts KF, Bateman BT, Desai RJ, Hernandez-Diaz S, Rough K, Mogun H, Kerzner LS, Davis JM, Stover M, Bartels D. Risk of neonatal drug withdrawal after intrauterine co-exposure to opioids and psychotropic medications: cohort study. BMJ. 2017;358:j3326.
3. Bateman BT, Hernandez-Diaz S, Rathmell JP, Seeger JD, Doherty M, Fischer MA, Huybrechts KF. Patterns of opioid utilization in pregnancy in a large cohort of commercial insurance beneficiaries in the United States. J Am Soc Anesthesiologists. 2014;120:1216–24.
4. Desai RJ, Hernandez-Diaz S, Bateman BT, Huybrechts KF. Increase in prescription opioid use during pregnancy among Medicaid-enrolled women. Obstet Gynecol. 2014;123:997.
5. Patrick SW, Davis MM, Lehmann C, Cooper WO. Increasing incidence and geographic distribution of neonatal abstinence syndrome: United States 2009 to 2012. J Perinatol. 2015;35:650–5.
6. Swaminathan N. Can HGH Reverse Brain Damage in Drug Addicts? In Scientific American, vol. https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/can-hgh- reverse-brain-dam/. Online; 2008.
7. Svensson A-L, Bucht N, Hallberg M, Nyberg F. Reversal of opiate-induced apoptosis by human recombinant growth hormone in murine foetus primary hippocampal neuronal cell cultures. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2008;105: 7304–8.
Arndt and Jones Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy (2018) 13:11 Page 2 of 3
8. Dublin S, Walker RL, Gray SL, Hubbard RA, Anderson ML, Yu O, Crane PK, Larson EB. Prescription opioids and risk of dementia or cognitive decline: a prospective cohort study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63:1519–26.
9. Kamboj S, Conroy L, Tookman A, Carroll E, Jones L, Curran H. Effects of immediate-release opioid on memory functioning: a randomized-controlled study in patients receiving sustained-release opioids. Eur J Pain. 2014;18: 1376–84.
10. Tibboel D. Long-term effects of early exposure to stress, pain, opioids and anaesthetics on pain sensitivity and neurocognition. Curr Pharm Des. 2017; 23:5879–86.
11. Mellon RD, Simone AF, Rappaport BA. Use of anesthetic agents in neonates and young children. Anesth Analg. 2007;104:509–20.
• We accept pre-submission inquiries
• Our selector tool helps you to find the most relevant journal
• We provide round the clock customer support
• Convenient online submission
• Thorough peer review
• Inclusion in PubMed and all major indexing services
• Maximum visibility for your research
Submit your manuscript at www.biomedcentral.com/submit
Submit your next manuscript to BioMed Central and we will help you at every step:
Arndt and Jones Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy (2018) 13:11 Page 3 of 3
- Case 1
- Case 2
- Publisher’s Note
- Author details
- References
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.