What are two new things you learned from completing the lab activities? How will you apply this knowledge to your future PMHNP practice? Use information from these arti
What are two new things you learned from completing the lab activities?
How will you apply this knowledge to your future PMHNP practice?
Use information from these articles.
page 1
www.nncionline.org
WHAT TO SAY WHEN PATIENTS ASK
Perpetual Hunger: The Neurobiological Consequences of Long-Term Opioid Use Jane Eisen, MD
INTRODUCTION/CLINICAL VIGNETTE (Group, 2-5 minutes)
Ms. Roman is a 26 yo single woman with long-standing chronic pain accompanied by long-standing opioid use. She has had many work-ups to identify the etiology of her diffuse pain symptoms without a clear cause identi- fied. Because of her pain, and concomitant prescribed opioid use, she has had poor functioning over the past 5 years. After a brief stint in LA when she took acting classes 3 years ago, she has largely been in her parents’ home, with minimal socializing and no work efforts. She reports difficulties with interpersonal relationships in addition to her chronic pain, feeling that she is frequently treated poorly or ignored by friends and acquaintances, so has increasingly isolated herself.
ROLE PLAY #1 (Pairs, 5 minutes)
One partner should play the role of patient and the other should play the role of clinician and attempt to answer the patient’s questions: 1) It seems that I am more sensitive to everything
compared to how I was before this huge pain prob- lem started, despite being on pain medication. Why is that?
2) All I can think about is my pain – everyone tells me to ignore it – I am on pain medication, I know. But I just can’t; it’s too intense.
3) Even if my pain weren’t limiting me so much, I’m always down because I keep getting hurt! I don’t think there’s any therapy that can fix my being mistreated by my supposed friends.
REVIEW NNCI RESOURCE (Individual, 10 minutes)
Perpetual Hunger: The Neurobiological Consequences of Long Term Opioid Use Biological Psychiatry Clinical Commentary by Tanner Bommersbach, David A. Ross, Joao P. De Aquino. ISSN: 0006-3223 Biological Psychiatry October 15, 2017; 82:e61–e63
DISCUSSION (Group, 15 minutes)
What were the key take-home points you learned from this clinical commentary?
ROLE PLAY #2 (Pairs, 5 minutes)
Role play a second time, again with one partner playing the role of patient and the other playing the role of clinician, and attempt to answer the patient’s questions, incorporating any additional information you learned from the reading: 1) It seems that I am more sensitive to everything
compared to how I was before this huge pain prob- lem started, despite being on pain medication. Why is that?
2) All I can think about is my pain – everyone tells me to ignore it – I am on pain medication, I know. But I just can’t; it’s too intense.
3) Even if my pain weren’t limiting me so much, I’m always down because I keep getting hurt! I don’t think there’s any therapy that can fix my being mistreated by my supposed friends.
WRAP UP (Individual, 2-5 minutes)
The keysheet includes key take-home points and example answers to our patient’s question(s) – of course, what’s written in the keysheet is only one possible explanation; your exact answers will vary, depending on your style and your individual patient. If time allows, take a few minutes now to read the keysheet and see how it matches up with the discussion and role play you’ve just had.
You might also use this time to consider what one thing you will do differently, in either your clinical or teaching encounters, as a result of this experience.
The keysheet also includes another section, "What Else To Say", which identifies additional learning opportunities that you may be interested in reviewing after this session.
*Note that time estimates listed are for a 45-minute session, but can be adjusted according to your session length.
page 2
www.nncionline.org
ANSWER SHEET
What were key take-home points you learned from this piece? We are beginning to understand neurobiological
mechanisms that lead to the well-described phe- nomenon of hyperalgesia. Chronic opioid exposure reduces the pain threshold by affecting both central and peripheral pain pathways:
• Chronic opioid use leads to inflammatory cytokine abnormalities ,which affects bottom-up nociceptive transmission.
• Chronic opioid use affects a number of changes to the mu, NDMA, and serotonin receptors centrally.
Additionally, based on neuroimaging studies, we can see that central processing of pain over time shifts from sensory to emotional and cognitive brain areas so that people with chronic pain overly focus on their experience of pain, leading to a sense of profound helplessness.
Another important change in chronic opioid use is the alteration of incentive salience whereby neuro- plastic changes in cognitive and reward circuits lead to sensitization to (or attentional bias towards) opioid and pain cues, at the expense of healthy interactions in the environment.
A third central change noted in those with chronic opioid use in the development of chronic emotional distress and dysregulation (hyperkatifeia), which further leads to opioid-seeking behaviors to alleviate negative affective states.
Individual variability regarding symptoms in one of 3 domains affected by chronic opioid use – pain sensitivity, incentive salience, and affective distress
– has been documented by neuroimaging, neuropsy- chological testing, and clinical studies.
Future directions for addiction medicine are to develop more personalized approaches, given this variability, to target treatments based on which specific domains are most affected.
Finally, given the long-standing brain changes with chronic opioid use, a chronic illness model, analo- gous to diabetes or hypertension, may have clinical utility, particularly in light of the potentially lethal consequences of relapse.
1) It seems that I am more sensitive to the slightest pain compared to how I was before this huge pain problem started, despite being on pain medication. Why is that?
Even though opioids are powerful at relieving pain at first, over time they change the “thermostats” for how the body regulates mood and sensitivity to painful stimuli so that most people with opioid dependence unfortunately develop exactly what you describe – hypersensitivity to pain. This is an effect of the opioids you are taking and it happens because of complex changes that take place in your brain in a number of neuroreceptors including the mu-opioid receptor.
It also happens because the pain signals from your body are amplified by chronic opioid use’s effects on the inflammatory system.
2) All I can think about is my pain – everyone tells me to ignore it – I am on pain medication, I know. But I just can’t; it’s too intense.
Several things happen to people in your situation with chronic opioid use and chronic pain. First, they develop actual increased pain sensitivity. Second, over time, the brain becomes overly-attuned to anything having to do with pain, snatching your attention and preventing you from focusing on anything else. Another factor here is that the focus on the sensations of pain shifts to the emotional and thinking centers of the brain so that your thoughts and feelings become overly focused on pain. All these brain changes help explain why it’s so hard for you to concentrate on anything other than your pain.
3) Even if my pain weren’t limiting me so much, I’m always down because I keep getting emotionally hurt! I don’t think there’s any therapy that can fix my being mistreated by my supposed friends.
We also now know that chronic opioid use leads to a dysregulation of moods so that negative states are intensified and positive states are minimized even long after people stop using opioids. No wonder you are sensitive to interactions with others! The slightest negative interaction is hugely amplified and any positive interactions are completely minimized. Psychotherapy can really help address the kind of interpersonal sensitivity you are describing.
WTS: WHAT TO SAY WHEN PATIENTS ASK
page 3
www.nncionline.org
WHAT ELSE TO KNOW
Below is a cartoon of the neural circuitry involved with the negative emotional state of withdrawal/negative affect stage including the reward circuitry (ventral teg- mental area and nucleus accumbens) and the extended amygdala with resulting changes in neurotransmitters.
From a clinical perspective, a focus on understanding
the reward deficit and/or stress surfeit component of the withdrawal/negative affect stage of opioid use disorder can also inform which behavioral treatments may be more effective in moderate to severe opioid use disorder. For example, versions of cognitive behavioral therapy that address coping mechanisms for stress and pain (physical and affective) may be more important than refinements in contingency management.*
Neural circuitry associated with the negative emo- tional state of the withdrawal/negative affect stage. (A) Extended amygdala and within-system neuroadap- tations. Note the loss of dopamine and opioid peptide function in ventral tegmental area–nucleus accumbens circuitry, with a hypothesized contribution of the habenula that suppresses neuron activity in the ventral tegmental area (inset panel). (B) Extended amygdala and between-system neuroadaptations.
Note the gain of stress neurotransmitter and neuro- modulator function and loss of antistress neurotrans- mitter and neuromodulator function throughout the neurocircuitry of the extended amygdala (inset panel). The extended amygdala is composed of several basal forebrain structures, including the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, the central nucleus of the amygdala,
and possibly a transition area in the medial portion (shell) of the nucleus accumbens.
ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; BNST, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis; CeA, central nucleus of the amygdala; DA, dopamine; DS, dorsal striatum; dlPFC, dorsolat- eral prefrontal cortex; GABA, gamma-aminobutyric acid; GP, globus pallidus; HPC, hippocampus; LDT, laterodorsal tegmentum; NAC, nucleus accumbens; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; PAG, periaqueductal gray; PPT, pedunculopontine tegmentum; Thal, thalamus; vlPFC, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex; VTA, ventral tegmental area. [Adapted from: 1) Koob GF (2008). A role for brain stress systems in addiction. Neuron 59:11–34. 2) George O, Koob GF (2013). Control of craving by the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:4165–4166.]
WTS: WHAT TO SAY WHEN PATIENTS ASK
page 4
www.nncionline.org
* Review Neurobiology of Opioid Addiction: Opponent Process, Hyperkatifeia, and Negative Reinforcement George F. Koob https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.05.023
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Dr. Eisen is the Clinical Director of the McLean Hospital Depression and Anxiety Division. The National Neuroscience Curriculum Initiative is supported by the National Institutes of Health Grant Nos. R25 MH08646607S1 and R44 MH115546-01 ©National Neuroscience Curriculum Initiative.
WTS: WHAT TO SAY WHEN PATIENTS ASK
,
page 1
www.nncionline.org
FACILITATOR’S GUIDE
“Cut and Paste” Clinical Pathology: Neurodevelopmental Disorders Tara Buck, MD, and Ashley Walker, MD
OVERVIEW
A central challenge in teaching and learning neuroscience is the depth and complexity of the material. In this regard, we are in good company: for thousands of years and in various contexts people have faced similar challenges. The ancient Greeks are well known for having developed mnemonic de- vices to assist with complex memory tasks. A lovely review of some of these techniques can be found in Joshua Foer’s article: “Secrets of a Mind-Gamer: How I trained my brain and became a world-class memory athlete” (http://www.nytimes.com/ interactive/2011/02/20/magazine/mind-secrets.html?_r=0). As Foer describes, memory can be significantly enhanced “by con- structing a building in the imagination and filling it with imagery” of what needs to be recalled. This imagined building has since been referred to as a “memory palace”. Moreover, in developing a memory palace, “the funnier, lewder and more bizarre, the better.” He goes on to explain, “When we see in everyday life things that are petty, ordinary and banal, we generally fail to remember them…. But if we see or hear something exceptionally base, dishonorable, extraordinary, great, unbelievable or laugh- able, that we are likely to remember for a long time.” The following module is designed to apply these principles to the teaching and learning of neuroscience topics. This particular “Cut and Paste” session is designed around neurodevelopmen- tal disorders, but the same approach could be used for other disorders or for a wide range of other topics in neuroscience. Of note, we would emphasize the dual role that such a session can play: both for enhancing memory for specific content and also helping train participants in the process of applying mnemonic devices – the more one practices these techniques, the easier they become to apply to future content! Goal: To create a space in which participants can apply classic mnemonic approaches to enhance memory for core neuroscience content. Learning Objective: By the end of this session, participants will be able to describe clinical presentations of major neurode- velopmental disorders.
RESOURCES REQUIRED
• Facilitator • Table space to work • Magazines featuring people (models, celebrities) • Paper or poster board (14in. x 22in. is a pretty good size) • Colored pencils or markers • Scissors • Tape or glue sticks
SESSION OUTLINE
This session is intended for a 45-60 minute class period depending on how many collages each learner makes. The session can be expanded to 90 minutes by having participants also discuss recommendations for treatment of their assigned syndromes. Learners may work individually, or in small groups. 1. “Cut and Paste”
• Working individually (or in pairs), each person is assigned one of the clinical syndromes in Table 1. It is not neces- sary for all syndromes to be assigned in class, especially in smaller groups or shorter timeframes.
• Each learner is instructed to put together a collage of pictures from magazines that demonstrate the “clinical features” of the illness (shown in column 2).
2. “Show and Tell”
• Learners share with the large group their collage and how the features of their syndrome are represented.
• Alternatively, collages can be displayed at the front of the classroom and learners can guess which is which, based on the clinical features represented.
• In order to consolidate learning, in large groups where multiple participants have the same assignment, we recommend that once the collage from one of the syndromes is presented, that everyone with that same assigned syndrome shows their collage as well.
ADDITIONAL/ALTERNATIVE LEARNING OPPORTUNITIES:
In Table 1, participants will also find a description of the pathology and brain changes that are known to be associated with each neurodevelopmental condition. This section could be reviewed by participants on their own or in a classroom set- ting as a companion “Find it, Draw it, Know it” module, in which participants draw and map out the associated pathology on pictures of the brain. Table 1 also includes links to educational videos of patients with some of the clinical features described (see links in first column). These could be shown in the classroom setting to con- solidate learning. Alternatively participants could be instructed to review them on their own outside of class time. This session can be run with both residents and child psy- chiatry fellows, and could also be used for medical students. Advanced trainees could be further challenged to provide a description of treatment recommendations when presenting their collages to the group. As there are multiple profiles that can cause intellectual dis- ability, a session (especially shorter or with fewer participants) could be run focusing solely on intellectual disability (Table 2). Each participant/pair creating a collage could be assigned a specific etiology of intellectual disability.
page 2
www.nncionline.org
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS:
Drs. Buck and Walker are Assistant Professors of Psychiatry, as well as Associate Program Directors for the Child and General Psychiatry training programs, respectively, at the University of Oklahoma School of Community Medicine in Tulsa. The National Neuroscience Curriculum Initiative is a collaborative effort with the American Association of Directors of Psychiatric Residency Training (AADPRT) and the American Psychiatric Association (APA) Council on Medical Education and Lifelong Learning and receives grant support from the NIH (R25 MH101076 02S1 and R25 MH086466 07S1) ©National Neuroscience Curriculum Initiative.
REFERENCES:
1. Amat JA, Bronen RA, Saluja S, et al. Increased number of subcortical hyperintensities on MRI in children and ado- lescents with Tourette’s syndrome, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry, 2006; 163(6):1106-1108.
2. Bauman ML, Kemper TL. Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism: a review and future directions. Int J Dev Neurosci., 2005; 23:183-187.
3. Campbell LE, Stevens A, Daly E, et al. A comparative study of cognition and brain anatomy between two neurodevelop- mental disorders: 22q11.2 deletion syndrome and Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 2009; 47(4):1034-1044.
4. Capone G, Goyal P, Ares W, Lannigan E. Neurobehavioral disorders in children, adolescents, and young adults with Down syndrome. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet., 2006;142C(3):158-72.
5. Chiang M, Reiss A, Lee A., et al. 3D pattern of brain ab- normalities in Williams syndrome visualized using tensor- based morphometry. Neuroimage, 2007; 36:1096–1109.
6. Clark MM, Plante E. Morphology of the inferior frontal gy- rus in developmentally language-disordered adults. Brain and Language, 1998; 61(2): 288-303.
7. Dickstein SG, Bannon K, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 2006; 47:1051-1062.
8. Eckert MA, Galaburda AM, Mills DL, et al. The neurobiol- ogy of Williams syndrome: Cascading influences of visual system impairment? Cell Mol Life Sci, 2006; 63:1867-1875.
9. Jernigan TL, Bellugi U, Sowell E, Doherty S, Hesselink JR. Cerebral morphologic distinctions between Williams and Down syndromes. Archives of Neurology, 1993; 50:186–91.
10. Kalanithi PS, Zheng W, DiFiglia M, et al. Altered parval- bumin-positive neuron distribution in basal ganglia of individuals with Tourette syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2005; 102(37):13307-13312.
11. Leckman JF, Bloch MH, Smith ME, Larabi D, Hampson M. Neurobiological Substrates of Tourette’s Disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 2010; 20(4):237-247.
12. Lee AD, Leow AD, Lu A, et al. 3D pattern of brain abnormali- ties in Fragile X Syndrome visualized using tensor-based morphometry. NeuroImage, 2007; 34(3):924-938.
13. Leonard CM, Lombardino LJ, Walsh K, et al. Anatomical risk factors that distinguish dyslexia from SLI predict reading skill in normal children. Journal of communication disorders, 2002; 35:501-31.
14. Martin A, Volkmar FR, Lewis’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: A Comprehensive Textbook. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer Health, 2011. Print.
15. Pinter JD, Eliez S, Schmitt JE, Capone GT, Reiss AL. Neuroanatomy of Down’s Syndrome: A High-Resolution MRI Study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 2001; 158(10):1659-1665.
16. Price GR, Ansari D. “Dyscalculia: Characteristics, Causes, and Treatments.” Numeracy, 2013; 6(1): Article 2. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.1.2
17. Reiss AL, Eliez S, Schmitt JE, et al. IV. Neuroanatomy of Williams syndrome: a high-resolution MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2000; 12(Suppl 1):65–73.
18. Shaywitz BA, Lyon GR, Shaywitz SE. The role of functional magnetic resonance imaging in understanding reading and dyslexia. Dev Neuropsychol., 2006; 30:613-632.
19. Singer HS. Motor stereotypies. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 2009; 16:77-81.
20. Stern E, Silbersweig DA, Chee KY, et al. A functional neuro- anatomy of tics in Tourette syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 2000; 57(8):741-748.
21. Vellutino FR, Fletcher JM, Snowling MJ, Scanlon DM. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 2004; 45:2-40.
22. Wilhelm CJ, Guizzetti M. Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders: An Overview from the Glia Perspective. Front. Integr. Neurosci., 2016; 9(65).
23. Wolff JJ, Gu H, Gerig G, et al. Differences in white matter fiber tract development present from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism. Am J Psychiatry, 2012; 169:589-600.
24. Yamada et al. Brain developmental abnormalities in Prader-Willi syndrome detected by diffusion tensor imag- ing. Pediatrics; 2006 Aug ; 118(2) e442-8
25. Zwicker JG, Missiuna C, Boyd LA. Neural correlates of developmental coordination disorder: a review of hypoth- eses. J Child Neurology, 2009; 24(10):1273-81.
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.