If you are new to behavior analysis or behaviorism, discuss if and how your views have changed. If you are familiar with behavior analysis, indicate whether you are a methodological
If you are new to behavior analysis or behaviorism, discuss if and how your views have changed. If you are familiar with behavior analysis, indicate whether you are a methodological or radical behaviorist. In the subject line, enter a summation of what best describes your viewpoint (e.g., stronger behaviorist, psychologist, radical behaviorist, etc.). Be sure to reference the scholarly material presented as well as refer to a specific topic of interest. Remember, although you are exploring your viewpoints, continue to use APA style, scientific writing, and avoid the overuse of the first person.
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1973, 6, 209-217 NUMBER 2 (SUMMER 1973)
SOCIAL CONTROL OF FORM DIVERSITY AND THE EMERGENCE OF NEW FORMS IN CHILDREN'S BLOCKBUILDING1
ELIZABETH M. GoETZ AND DoNALD M. BAER2
UNIVERSI1Y OF KANSAS
The blockbuilding behavior of three preschool girls was analyzed in terms of the forms manifest in any completed block construction, and found to contain few different forms in baseline sessions. Social reinforcement, given contingent on the production of any form not previously constructed within the current session (i.e., every first appearance of any form within a session was reinforced but no subsequent appearances of that form within that session were), increased the number of different forms built per sessions. Social reinforcement, given for all second and later appearances within the session, de creased the number of different forms built per session. Furthermore, it was found that new forms (forms never seen before in the child's total prior sequence of blockbuilding sessions) emerged at higher rates during periods of reinforcement of different forms (first appearances) than during periods of baseline or reinforcement of same forms (sec ond and later appearances).
Virtually every nursery, preschool, and day care center is equipped with a collection of blocks, usually incorporating a variety of shapes and sizes. Use of these blocks is ordinarily a frequent activity of the majority of preschool children, and such play is widely believed to be educational, contributing to the child's concepts of space, form, mathematics (cf. Cuisenaire rods), balance, leverage, and visual esthetics. The possibility of such a contribution does not appear to have been subjected to experimental analysis and verification, probably because the
1A preliminary version of this study has been pub lished in A New Direction fM Education: Behavior Analysis, Vol. I, edited by E. A. Ramp and B. L. Hopkins, (Lawrence: University of Kansas Printing Service, 1971). Supported in part by the University of Kansas Center for Research in Early Childhood Education, in association with the National Program for Research in Early Childhood Education, CEMREL, Inc., and the U. S. Office of Education. (Grant numbers OEC 3-7-070706-3118 and OEC 0-70-4152(607}). We thank Ms. Susan Young, Mr. Charles Burns, and Mr. Lawrence Russell for assis tance in obtaining the measurements.
2Reprints may be obtained from either author, Department of Human Development, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas 66044.
outcomes are not well specified in objectively measurable behavioral terms. Even without an empirical basis, though, it might well be guessed that if blockbuilding were to yield such a contribution, the block play itself would need to be diverse rather than limited, repetitive, or stereotyped. If the guess is a good one, then diverse block play becomes a behavioral goal in itself. Significantly, it is one that can readily be given behavioral definition and thereby be sub jected to experimental programming, such as by external reinforcement contingencies. If the guess is not a good one, the deliberate develop ment of diverse blockbuilding may nevertheless be taken as an experimental goal, so as to ask, if it is developed, what else results? The present study was undertaken to demonstrate the possi bility of reinforcing an objectively specifiable aspect of children's blockbuilding that would yield results that might readily be labelled diverse or (less readily, no doubt} even creative. To the extent that such behavior is valued in itself, these procedures then contribute to an educational technology capable of producing it; to the extent that the value of such behavior is questioned, these procedures contribute to the
209
210 EUZABETH M. GOETZ and DONALD M. BAER
possibility of experimental examination of any other results of its deliberate development.
METHOD
Setting
The study took place in a university preschool classroom, specifically in the blockbuilding area of the classroom ( characterized by a clear expanse of flat floor bordered by shelves con taining the blocks). Each subject was invited by the teacher to play with the blocks in this area, alone with her and an observer, stationed a few feet away. These invitations were made every few days, and were invariably accepted. The blockbuilding session took place during a time of day when the block area was free of all other children (who were engaged in curricular ac tivity outdoors). The session continued until the child said she was finished or until all the blocks available had been used and their rearrange ment had stopped, and the child agreed when the teacher asked, "Are you finished?". Usually, the child made a single construction per session; occasionally, several separate constructions were made in a session, in which case all were con sidered in the data analysis of that session.
Subjects
Three girls, each 4-yr old, had little else in common apart from an absence of well-devel oped blockbuilding skills. This deficit had been remarked on informally by the classroom staff during the months of school preceding the study, and was formally examined in a baseline period at the outset of the study. Staff comments emphasized that these girls' blockbuilding efforts were either devoid of construction ( the blocks merely being laid out in like-shaped or like sized groups), or primitive (characterized by repetition of the same structure–e.g., the same "casde"-in every successive construction). One subject came from a low-income family; the other two represented highly enriched, intellec tually and culturally stimulating family situ ations.
Behavior Definitions and Recording
Child behaviors. The basic child behaviors of blockbuilding were defined according to their products, block forms. In general, form referred to various uses of two or more blocks to create a specified shape or function. Specifically, an arbitrary but frequently seen 20 such forms were defined, as listed in Table 1. Most of these forms could be constructed from a wide variety of blocks; only a few are defined in terms of specific pieces ("circles", "S", "X", and "inter face"). Note that some of these definitions re quire that the block collection contain rec tangular solids of various length-width-thickness ratios and arc-shaped and V-shaped pieces, as well as the more familiar cubes.
A form diversity score was defined as the number of these 20 forms appearing at least once in any session's construction(s).
A new forms score was defined as the number of these 20 forms appearing in a given session's construction that had not appeared in any prior construction by that child (in previous sessions of blockbuilding) recorded within the study. The new forms measure was not scored for the child's first block construction(s) in the first session of the study, in that all forms ap pearing in that session would have to be con sidered new. Instead, the number of forms appearing in the first construction(s) of the first session of the study was taken as the child's baseline of forms; new forms were then con sidered forms other than those and were scored from the second session's construction(s) on.
Recording of each construction was done photographically with a Polaroid camera. A series of photographs was taken of each ses sion's construction(s), from all sides and angles necessary for complete display of its structure. (The use of a Polaroid camera was considered important, in that each photograph could be examined for adequacy of exposure, focusing, scope, and clarity almost immediately; deficient ones then could be replaced or augmented by additional photographs before the construction
211 FORM DIVERSITY AND NEW FORMS
Table 1
Definition of 20 Block Forms
FENCE: any two or more blocks placed side by side in contiguity; if not contiguous, then any three or more blocks placed at regularly spaced intervals in a straight line.
STORY: any two or more blocks placed one atop another, the upper block(s) resting solely upon the lower.
RAMP: a block leaned against another, or a tri angular block placed contiguous to another, to sim ulate a ramp.
PILLAR: any story in which the lowest block is at least twice as tall as it is wide.
POST: any story in which the lowest block is at least twice as wide and half as rall as the upper block(s).
TOWER: any story of two or more blocks, each of which is at least twice as tall as it is wide.
ROOF: two or more slat-shaped blocks placed flat and side by side atop at least two supports.
FLOOR: an inverted roof. BALANCE: any story in which the upper block 1s
at least four times as wide as the lower. ELABORATED BALANCE: any balance in which
both ends of the upper block contain additional blocks.
ENCLOSURE: any arrangement of fences which encloses an open area, with or without "gate".
SUBDIVISION: two or more enclosures in con tiguity with at least one common fence.
ARCH: any placement of a block atop two lower blocks not in contiguity.
STORIED ARCH: an arch built atop another arch. ADJUNCT: two or more forms connected by a
fence; at least one of the forms must be an en closure, subdivision, or roof.
CIRCLE: arrangement of four arc-shaped blocks in contiguity to form a circle.
"S": arrangement of four arc-shaped blocks in con tiguity as two half-circles to simulate an s.
"X": arrangement of two V-shaped blocks in con tiguity to simulate an X.
INTERFACE: arrangement of any two blocks with curved contours to fit precisely together, such as circle into hole of doughnut-shaped block, or half circle into arc-shaped block.
SIMULATION: a construction of blocks which re sembles a real-life object and is explicitly labelled by child as such, usually a building, boat, or swimming pool.
was dismantled). These photographs were taken after the child had left the setting; the camera was not in evidence during the blockbuilding, and the child was presumably unaware of the fact of photographic recording.
Two judges independently examined each series of photographs, counting the number of forms appearing at least once in the series representing the session's construction. Their agreement in this counting of form diversity was compared and found to be 100% over all sessions of the study. In that the new forms score was derived directly from these counts ( comparing the identities of the forms found in a given construction to those found in all previous constructions), there was similarly 100% agreement between the judges on that score as well.
The duration of each session was recorded by the observer with a stopwatch. The session was defined as beginning when the first block was set down, and as ending either when the child
said it was or when the child replied affirma tively to a teacher question about completion.
Teacher behaviors. An observer silently watched the blockbuilding interaction from a few feet away and recorded teacher behavior and its contingency with the child's production of forms. Specifically, the observer recorded each successive new form built by the child in that day's construction, whether the teacher attended to that form production or not, whether the at tention was enthusiastic and approving, and the duration of the session. These records were used primarily as a check on the teacher's efficiency in carrying out the social contingencies required by the experimental design, rather than as a direct measure of the child's behaviors in producing forms. (The photographs were pre ferred as likely to yield higher accuracy, in that they could be examined at leisure many times over, whereas the observer was required to judge all blockbuilding behaviors immediately as they occurred). In addition, the teacher herself main-
212 ELIZABETH M. GOETZ and DONALD M. BAER
tained similar records, as the blockbuilding proceeded. Comparison of the teacher's records and the observer's showed agreement on 95% of the events recorded, over the study, and also showed that 95% of the teacher's attention was supplied in the contingencies required by the experimental design, across the study.
Procedures
Baseline: no reinforcement. During the first three to five sessions (varying with the subject), the teacher sat by the child as she built with the blocks, watching closely but quietly, displaying neither criticism nor enthusiasm about any particular use of the blocks. At the end of each baseline session, the teacher expressed her ap preciation of the child's total effort and con ducted her back to the rest of the classroom group (usually in the play yard outside), and then returned to photograph the construction(s). Baseline sessions were continued until inspection of the child's daily form diversity scores showed a stable enough level to justify experimental programming. (The new forms score was not considered).
Reinforcement of different forms. After the baseline sessions, the teacher began a program of social reinforcement of new forms. In these sessions, the teacher remarked with interest, enthusiasm, and delight every time that the child placed and/or rearranged blocks so as to create a form that had not appeared previously in that session's construction(s). (The form might well have appeared in any previous session's construction(s), but it would be reinforced in this session nevertheless). In other words, the teacher reinforced every first appearance of any form within the current session but no subse quent appearances of that form within that session. The content of the teacher's remarks was designed to accomplish descriptive rein forcement: that is, they often indicated the dimension to be reinforced, such as, "Oh, that's very nice-that's different!". These procedures were continued for four or five sessions until clear evidence of increasing form diversity was
obtained, at which point the next experimental condition was implemented. (The new forms score was not considered).
Reinforcement of same forms. Experimental control of the increasing form diversity score was attempted by reversal of the direction of the social reinforcement contingency employed. Thus, for the next two to four sessions, the teacher continued to display interest, enthusiasm, and delight, but only at those times when the child placed and/or rearranged a block so as to create a repetition of a form already apparent in that session's construction(s). (This form need not have appeared in any previous session's construction(s); so long as it had appeared earlier in this session's construction(s), it would be reinforced.) Thus, no first usage of a form in a session was reinforced, but every second usage of that form and every usage thereafter within the session was. Again the content of praise was descriptive: it specified the dimensions (same ness) being reinforced (e.g., "How nice-an other arch!"). These sessions were continued until a clear decrease in form diversity was seen. (As ever, the new forms score was not considered).
Resumption of reinforcement of different forms. To conclude the experimental analysis and leave each child with the desired high level of diversity, reinforcement of different forms was resumed and continued for three to five sessions, until high levels of form diversity were seen. Procedures during this condition were identical to those used in the previous con dition of reinforcement of different forms, i.e., only first appearances of any form within the current session were reinforced.
Number of blocks to be used. The first two subjects, Sally and Kathy, were free to use any number of blocks, many or few, for each con struction. In fact, each girl tended to use a number of blocks roughly correlated with her form diversity score, across constructions. While the correlation was not high, it did allow the possibility that the increasing form diversity scores associated with reinforcement of different
213 FORM DIVERSITY AND NEW FORMS
n – NO REINFORCEMENT D – REINFORCE ONLY DIFFERENT FOAMS S- REINFORCE ONLY SAME FORMS
20
10
SALLY
J
0"'1———————
IL. 0
20
KATHY
MARY
• • •
I :, z-
10
•• o,_____________________
COOSTRUCTIONS Fig. I. The form diversity scores of three children in the course of block-building training. Initial points,
labelled as n's, represent scores produced when reinforcement was programmed only for different (non repetitive) forms; and points labelled as S's represent scores produced when reinforcement was programmed only for repetition of the same forms used previously that session .
214 ELIZABETH M. GOETZ and DONALD M. BAER
forms could represent a chance outcome of the increased opportunity to display different forms when using many blocks. Consequently, Mary's procedures were conducted as described above, but with one added element: Mary was told that she must use all of the blocks (53 10 number) each session. Invariably she did.
RESULTS
Form diversity. Each child's form diversity score, for each experimental session, is shown in Figure 1. It is apparent that the social con tingencies applied effectively controlled the development of form diversity: each child showed appreciable increases from her baseline (no reinforcement) levels during the two periods of reinforcement of different forms; and during the interpolated condition of reinforcement of same forms, each child's form diversity de creased toward her baseline range.
New forms. Each child's new forms score, for all sessions after the first (which was used to define old forms for all later sessions), is shown in Figure 2, cumulatively. That is, each bar represents the number of new forms displayed by the child so far in her sessions, since the first session. (The number of "new" forms seen in her first session was of course identical to the first point in each graph of Figure 1.) The increment each bar shows over the immediately preceding bar is thus the number of new forms that emerged in that session. Figure 2 shows that the emergence of new forms was largely restricted to periods in which different forms were being reinforced. Only three exceptions appear: Sally, in her second baseline session (first bar in Figure 2), showed one new form, and in her third session, another new form; Mary in her second baseline session (first bar in Figure 2), showed four. By contrast, Sally showed nine new forms during periods of rein forcement of different forms, and Mary dis played 14 during these periods. In Kathy's case, her 16 new forms emerged only during periods of reinforcement of different forms. In no child's
study did new forms appear during the period of reinforcement of same forms. On the average, across the three children, the rate of emergence of new forms during periods of reinforcement of different forms was 1.5 new forms per session; the rate of emergence of new forms during periods of baseline or reinforcement of same forms was 0.33 per session. These figures ex clude the first baseline session, as required by the definition of the new forms score.
Session durations. The mean duration of blockbuilding sessions was 16 min in Sally's case, 19 min in Kathy's, and 9 min in Mary's. However, when Sally was receiving reinforce ment for different forms, her mean duration was 23 min; in conditions of baseline and reinforce ment of same forms, her mean duration was 9 min. The comparable mean durations for Kathy were 22 min and 12 min, respectively; and for Mary, 10 min and 8 min. Thus, in general, per formance during reinforcement of different forms required more time than during baseline or the reinforcement of same forms.
DISCUSSION
The three children of this study showed in creasing form diversity when they received descriptive reinforcement for creating different forms, and decreasing form diversity when repe tition of similar forms was descriptively rein forced. Thus, the teacher techniques implicit in descriptive reinforcement were functional in the analysis of this behavior of preschool chil dren, as they have been in many other studies (e.g., Baer and Wolf, 1968).
Sally and Kathy were free to use many blocks or few for each construction, but Mary was re quired to use all the 5 3 blocks in each construc tion. Mary's results thus represent the number of different forms achieved per 5 3 blocks in each session, and cannot represent simply a better chance to achieve many forms during the condition when different forms were being rein forced. The similarity of Mary's results to Sally's and Kathy's suggests that the same process oper-
215 FORM DIVERSITY AND NEW FORMS
i No Reinforcement Reinforce Only Different Forms Reinforce Only Same Forms
SALLY20
"
~ ~
I t i
~
20
.. 12
I
4
0
KATHY
20 MARY
Successive Constructions
Fig. 2. The cumulative number of new forms ( forms never used before) produced by three children in the course of block-building training. All scores represent cumulative increments above the score produced in the first session ( not graphed). ( The increment each bar shows over the preceding bar is thus the number of new forms produced in that session.)
216 ELIZABETH M. GOETZ and DONALD M. BAER
ated in all three cases, independently of the number of blocks used per construction.
It seems clear that diversity of response, within this delimited sphere of activity, is readily modified by simple, everyday reinforcement con tingencies. This is in agreement with the earlier demonstrations by Maltzman (1960), who per formed a series of studies using verbal instruc tion and/or praise to increase original word associations in college students, and by Pryor, Haag, and O'Reilly (1969), who increased never-before-observed body movements in two porpoises, using food reinforcers. However, the Maltzman studies displayed their effects as changes in the mean scores of various subject groups, which is certainly valuable but never theless actuarial. Creativity could have its great est meaning as a description of an individual's behavior. The Pryor et al. study showed a de liberate development of novel or original swim ming gymnastics in each of its two porpoises, but had difficulty maintaining objective, reliable measurement of these behaviors as they became increasingly original, and did not offer an experi mental analysis of the procedures necessary to this development. (Furthermore, creativity in the porpoise, while intriguing, may reasonably be doubted as an analogue of any similar process in man or child).
The generality of the ease of training dis played in the present study of course remains to be seen. Furthermore, whether or not to attribute the effect simply to a reinforcement mechanism remains to be seen as well. In this study, the content of the verbal reinforcement described the dimensions at issue, i.e., diversity as opposed to similarity of forms to be con structed. Analysis of the separate functions of description and reinforcement remains to be accomplished, although its value is more theo retical than applied. It is likely that the functions of description without praise and of praise with out description are different in different children. In particular, it may well be supposed that for some children, either will be sufficient without the other, but that for other children, the mix
of the two will be more effective than either alone. If so, then for applied purposes, a package of the two is probably the best technique to apply to children in general. The present study, of course, testifies to the effectiveness of the package, not to its components.
It may be noted that the package was applied to form diversity directly, and only to form diversity; change in the new forms score and in session durations are thus collateral changes, rather than direct results of the descriptive con tingencies applied to each new (or each same) form. Nevertheless, these changes, taken to gether, suggest that children receiving descrip tive reinforcement merely for form diversity within a session do display an emergence of new forms across sessions, and take more time in their blockbuilding as they do so. The descrip tive content of teacher praise specified "differ ence" (or "sameness"), but did not include any instruction in how to construct any form. For these reasons, the total pattern of behavior change seen in these three children might well be labelled "creative", pursuant to a discussion of the definitional problems inherent in the term.
It has never been easy to define creativity (cf. Crutchfield, 1965; Ghiselin, 1955; Guilford, 1959; Stein, 1953). However, one definition is attractive on its face and also lends itself to objective measurement and specific environ mental programming. This definition equates creativity to novel or original behavior, i.e., behavior that the subject of study has not dis played before in his present setting, or behavior that his group, class, or culture has not displayed before in that setting (Maltzman, 1960; Pryor et al., 1969). The phrase, "not displayed be fore", implies a criterion of time: not displayed since when? Clearly, this definition of creativity confers maximum creativity on those behaviors that have never been displayed before-mean ing never before within the limits of the ob server's memory (or within the limits of his recording system, or his culture's recording system). Less creativity is suggested for behaviors
217 FORM DIVERSITY AND NEW FORMS
that have not been displayed since more recent times than that, such as "since yesterday". Never theless, if the goal is to foster creativity through direct training, a lesser criterion of creativity may be necessary. To reinforce only those re sponses that have never been displayed before may result in an extremely thin schedule of rein forcement, which will often be an ineffective one. In the present study, the target of training was behavior not displayed previously in that session by the child. This short-term criterion of creativity allowed a realistic reinforcement schedule, and success with the short-term cri terion was associated with a modest collateral development of behavior consistent with a long term criterion, as evidenced by the new forms score. Thus, it may well be that a short-term criterion, important on pragmatic grounds, may result in long-term outcomes of both pragmatic value and theoretical significance. Meanwhile, the definition of "creativity" is no less arbitrary than it has ever been, but one facet of that ar bitrariness has been subjected to experimental analysis, within the obvious limits of this study.
REFERENCES
Baer, D. M. and Wolf, M. M. The reinforcement contingency in preschool and remedial education. In R. D. Hess and R. M. Baer, (Eds.), Early edu cation: Cu"ent theory, research, and practice. Chicago: Aldine, 1968. Pages 119-130.
Crutchfield, R. S. Creative thinking in children: its teaching and testing. In H. Brim, R. S. Crutch field, and W. Holtzman, (Eds.), Intelligence: Perspectives. New York: Harcourt Brace and World, 1965. Pages 33-64.
Ghiselin, B. The creative process. New York: New American Library, 1955.
Guliford, J. P. Traits of creativity. In H. H. Ander son, (Ed.), Creativity and its cultivation. New York: Harpers, 1959. Pp. 142-161.
Stein, M. E. Creativity and culture. Journal, of Psy chology, 1953, 36, 311-322.
Maltzman, I. On the training of originality. Psy chological Review, 1960, 67, 229-242.
Pryor, K. W., Haag, R., and O'Reilly, J. The crea tive porpoise: training for novel behavior. Jour nal, of the Experimental, Anatysis of Behavior, 1969, 12, 653-661.
Received 29 November 1971 (Revision requested 3 February 1972.) (Final, acceptance 28 November 1972.)
,
289 COMMENTARY
ACHIEVING PARITY: THE ROLE OF AUTOMATIC REINFORCEMENT
DAVID C. PALMER
SMITH COLLEGE
The central insight of Horne and Lowe's article is the importance of the role played by the discriminative effects of one's speech upon one's self. Informed by this insight, Horne and Lowe provide a parsimonious and coherent interpretation of the behavior said to show equivalence relations, exploiting only established concepts of behavior analysis. I am enthusiastic about both the goals and ac complishments of the article and therefore will confine myself to suggesting an elabora tion or refinement of one or two ideas lightly covered by the authors, particularly the role of automatic reinforcement in the shaping of the speaker's behavior (cf. Skinner, 1957, p. 164, 1979, p. 283; Sundberg, 1980;Vaughan & Michael, 1982).
Speech is special in that we stimulate our selves in just the same way and at the same moment we stimulate others. This is a char acteristic not entirely shared by sign lan guage, because the appearance of a sign var ies with the location of the viewer; typically the speaker and the listener view signs from opposite sides. Although subtle, this differ ence should impair the acquisition of naming in sign language relative to that of speech, because listener behavior is under the control of stimuli that the speaker can never quite reproduce. Moreover, we might expect more idiosyncrasies, or accents, among signers than among speakers.
The auditory feedback from one's speech can play a special role for a speaker who is already a competent listener. Horne and Lowe allude to the discriminative control that such feedback exerts over orienting behavior and note that this control contributes greatly to our understanding of performance in re search on equivalence classes. They also refer to the potential reinforcing function that
Correspondence concerning this commentary should be addressed to the author at the Department of Psy chology, Smith College, Northampton, Massachusetts 01063 (E-mail: <a href='/cdn-cgi/l/email-protection' class='__cf_email__' data-cfemail='abcfc8dbcac7c6ced9ebd8c8c2cec5c8ce85d8c6c2dfc385cecfd
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.