Honor and Emotional Reactions Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct (apa.org) ?to read the APA ethical principles of p
Honor and Emotional Reactions
Click here Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct (apa.org) to read the APA ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
Using the Online Library, search the article, "A Bumpy Train Ride: A Field Experiment on Insult, Honor, and Emotional Reactions."
Pay special attention to the Method section. Summarize the purpose of the study. Think about the specific issues or points they should have covered in the debriefing session. Consider the APA ethical requirements. Write the specific things the investigators would have had to include during the debriefing session if the study had been conducted in the United States. Explain why the investigators would have to include these specific things. Compile your work in a 1- to 2-page Microsoft Word document.
Submission Details:
- Support your responses with examples.
- Cite any sources in APA format.
BRIEF REPORTS
A Bumpy Train Ride: A Field Experiment on Insult, Honor, and Emotional Reactions
Hans IJzerman and Wilco W. van Dijk VU University Amsterdam
Marcello Gallucci University of Milan–Bicocca
The present research examined the relationship between adherence to honor norms and emotional reactions after an insult. Participants were 42 Dutch male train travelers, half of whom were insulted by a confederate who bumped into the participant and made a degrading remark. Compared with insulted participants with a weak adherence to honor norms, insulted participants with a strong adherence to honor norms were (a) more angry, (b) less joyful, (c) less fearful, and (d) less resigned. Moreover, insulted participants with a strong adherence to honor norms perceived more anger in subsequent stimuli than not-insulted participants with a strong adherence to these norms. The present findings support a direct relationship among insult, adherence to honor norms, and emotional reactions.
Keywords: emotions, honor, insult, field experiment
Several societies in the Mediterranean area of Europe and the southern United States have been described as “honor cultures” (Caro Baroja, 1965; D. H. Fischer, 1989; Nisbett & Cohen, 1996; Pitt-Rivers, 1965, 1977; Rodriguez Mosquera, Manstead, & Fischer, 2000). Honor refers to people’s value both in their own eyes and in the eyes of others, and a distinctive feature of honor cultures is the extent to which one’s personal worth is determined interpersonally (Miller, 1993; Nisbett & Cohen, 1996; Rodriguez Mosquera et al., 2000; Stewart, 1994). Whereas an individual’s personal worth is important in both honor and individualistic cultures, the role of social esteem in determining one’s personal worth is more important in honor cultures than in individualistic cultures.
Research has shown that, compared with individuals from indi- vidualistic cultures, those from honor cultures attach more impor- tance to family-related values and social recognition (A. H. Fischer, Manstead, & Rodriguez Mosquera, 1999). Furthermore, it has been shown that such values are more important in honor cultures than in other cultures in shaping the experience and expression of emotions like pride, shame, and anger (Rodriguez
Mosquera et al., 2000). For example, in cultures where honor is more salient, attacks on one’s honor, as in the case of insults, appear to be a common anger-eliciting event. Moreover, the elic- itation of anger in attacks on one’s honor usually leads to hostility and retaliation against the perpetrator as a way of restoring one’s honor (Cohen & Nisbett, 1994, 1997; Cohen, Nisbett, Bowdle, & Schwarz, 1996; Nisbett & Cohen, 1996; Stewart, 1994). The southern United States has been regarded as a prime example of an honor culture in which affronts are met with violent retribution. It has been argued that adherence to honor norms might explain the observation that the American south is more violent than the American north (Cohen, 1996; Nisbett & Cohen, 1996). For in- stance, Southerners are more likely to agree that violence is ac- ceptable in defense of home and family and as a mechanism of social control; consequently, they are more likely to endorse vio- lence as a response to an affront. In honor cultures, “even small disputes become contests for reputation and social status” (Cohen et al., 1996, p. 945); thus, individuals are expected to defend their honor or reputation, even if this means that they have to fight or kill for it (Nisbett & Cohen, 1996; Pitt-Rivers, 1965).
In an intriguing series of experiments, Cohen et al. (1996) found support for their hypothesis that White males reared in the south- ern United States react differently to an affront than those reared in the north. Compared with Northerners, who were relatively unaf- fected by an insult, Southerners were (a) more likely to think that their masculine reputation was threatened, (b) more upset, (c) more physiologically and cognitively primed for aggression, and (d) more likely to engage in aggressive and dominant behavior. Cohen et al. suggest that participants’ strength of adherence to honor norms is the underlying mechanism for these effects. That is, Southerners are argued to have a stronger adherence to honor norms than Northerners and, consequently, react stronger and more aggressively to an affront.
In their research, Cohen et al. (1996) compared the emotional reactions toward an affront of Southerners with those of North-
Hans IJzerman and Wilco W. van Dijk, Department of Social Psychol- ogy, VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands; Marcello Gallucci, Department of Psychology, University of Milan–Bicocca, Milan, Italy.
Hans IJzerman and Wilco W. van Dijk contributed equally to the research and publication of this study and share first authorship. The specific order of these authors’ names was determined in mutual agree- ment. We thank Piotr Winkielman and two anonymous reviewers for valuable comments on an earlier version of this article. We thank William Djoko, Nicole van Elteren, Bas Kockmann, and Irma Oldenburg for their contributions to the present study.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Wilco W. van Dijk, Department of Social Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]
Emotion Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 7, No. 4, 869 – 875 1528-3542/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.869
869
erners in terms of a cultural mechanism based on inner represen- tations such as a script. The script in a cultural of honor, for example, as described by Cohen and Gelfand (2006), prescribes how an individual is to act when insulted. Indeed, Cohen et al. demonstrated that the cultural script concerning honor of South- erners and Northerners contains important differences. However, although scripts are essential to understanding cultural mecha- nisms, G. R. Smith and Semin (2004) suggested that individuals do not merely rely on scripts to perform action B when situation A occurs. To further comprehend such cultural mechanisms, one should examine an individual’s values as to how to cope with situation A at the moment of occurrence.
The fact that norms in Cohen et al. (1996) were not directly measured precludes establishing a direct relation among an insult, adherence to honor norms, and emotional reactions. In the present research, we aimed at providing empirical support for this direct relation. Using a field experiment, we intended to show that, after an insult and compared with individuals with a weak adherence to honor norms, individuals with a strong adherence to these norms react differently. Moreover, these different emotional reactions should be moderated by differences in the strength of adherence to honor norms. These anticipated findings would provide an impor- tant contribution to existing findings on the relation between honor and emotions.
In our research, we modified Cohen et al.’s (1996) experimental set-up, in which a confederate bumps into an unsuspecting partic- ipant and insults him, in two important ways. First, we measured participants’ strength of adherence to honor norms. Second, we used a field setting to stage the study in more ecologically natural circumstances. Moreover, we examined the effect of an insult on both participants’ immediate emotional reactions and their percep- tion of hostility in subsequent stimuli. We assessed participants’ emotional reactions by observing their facial expressions and body language after an insult and expected that participants with strong adherence to honor norms would be more likely to express emo- tional responses indicating aggression and hostility; that is, they would appear (a) more angry, (b) less joyful, (c) less fearful, and (d) less resigned. Subsequently, participants’ perception of hostil- ity in subsequent stimuli was assessed via a face-rating task, in which they had to indicate which emotion was being expressed in a series of photographs of emotional and neutral faces and how intensely this emotional expression was. This procedure allowed us to examine whether, after an insult, participants with a strong adherence to honor norms will perceive more hostility (a) in neutral stimuli, (b) in all stimuli, or (c) only in stimuli that also involve an affront or challenge.
Method
Participants and Design
A total of 42 male train travelers (mean age � 30.90 years, SD � 15.80) participated in the study on a voluntary basis. Participants were assigned to the experimental (insult) condition or control (no insult) condition according to a rule that was estab- lished before the experiment commenced: Of all male participants who entered, every other (odd number) participant was insulted (n � 21), whereas the remaining participants were not insulted.1
Materials and Procedure
The experiment was conducted on a local train on the Amsterdam–Rotterdam track (� 70 km). Confederates served as observers or insulters and were seated in different parts of one compartment of the train. The two observers each faced opposing ways and switched positions throughout the experiment. Both observers could hear everything participants said and could read their body language (although from different perspectives). Two male confederates of similar height and weight served alternatively as insulters. None of the confederates were fully aware of the purpose of the study. Participants were train travelers who entered the local train at different stops between Amsterdam and Rotter- dam. One of the two observers signaled when a participant was approaching. Subsequently, a confederate stood up, bumped into the participant, and added: “Hey, watch it!” (Hey, kijk eens uit joh!).2 Immediately after the bumping incident, the observers rated the participants’ emotional reactions on a 7-point scale (1 � not at all, 7 � very much). Both observers rated how angry, irritated, joyful, happy, nervous, fearful, and resigned participants appeared.
After participants were seated, they were asked to complete several questionnaires. First, they answered several demographical questions. Second, participants’ adherence to honor norms was assessed by asking them to what extent they agreed with nine items (1 � do not agree at all, 7 � totally agree) extracted from Rodriguez Mosquera, Manstead, and Fischer’s (2002) honor ques- tionnaire. These items included questions about family honor, respect, reputation, and humiliation in public (e.g., “My honor is my reputation”).3 Finally, participants were given a face-rating task in which they were asked to indicate which emotion was being expressed in a series of four photographs. For each of the four photographs, participants were asked to choose one of six options (happiness, sadness, anger, fear, surprise, or disgust) and to indi- cate the intensity of the expression on a 5-point scale (1 � not at all strong, 5 � very strong). Stimuli were black-and-white photo- graphs of emotional and neutral faces taken from the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces set (KDEF; Lundqvist, Flykt, & Öhman, 1998). These photographs were morphs of facial photographs of 37 different males expressing the same emotion: anger (KDEF code: MANS), sadness (MSAS), fear (MAFS), or neutral (MNES).
1 From each condition, 1 additional participant declined to participate in the experiment. Also, 3 participants indicated that their religious affiliation was Islam, whereas the majority were Christians or nondenominational (similar to national averages). Combined with confederates’ observations of participants, we were fully assured that there were not more than 3 nonnative Dutch participants. We included these participants in our anal- ysis because the results were equivalent with or without them. None of the participants was aware of the relationship between the insult and the experiment.
2 To prevent the situation from becoming too hostile, confederates refrained from using the offensive term “asshole” (klootzak) as used in Cohen et al.’s (1996) study. Confederates were also instructed to continue walking after the incident to minimize contact between him and the participant and not to challenge the participant any further.
3 We only chose nine items because we wanted to keep our questionnaire as short and concise as possible (in light of the short duration of partici- pants’ average train ride). Items 2, 7, 9, 11, 14, 17, 20, 24, and 26 were extracted from Rodriguez Mosquera et al.’s (2002) 27-item honor ques- tionnaire on the basis of their relevance to the present research.
870 BRIEF REPORTS
After all questions were completed, participants were thor- oughly debriefed and the insulter apologized to the individuals he insulted. Participants assigned to the control condition completed the same procedure without being insulted (see Appendix A for more details of our experimental set-up).
Results
Adherence to Culture-of-Honor Norms
A principal-component analysis on the nine honor items ex- tracted one factor with an eigenvalue of 5.55 (all other eigenval- ues � 1), explaining 61.6% of the variance.4 Participants’ scores on the nine items were averaged to obtain a score for their adherence to honor norms (Cronbach’s � � .92). There was no significant difference between conditions on adherence to honor norms, F(1, 31) � 1.00, p � .82, suggesting that self-perception of emotional reactions in relation to honor norms is unlikely to account for the obtained results.
Emotional Reactions
Observers’ ratings of emotional reactions5 were averaged to obtain a measure of emotional reactions.6 Ratings of anger and irritation were averaged to obtain a measure of anger (� � .92), ratings of joy and happiness were averaged to obtain a measure of joy (� � .86), and ratings of fear and nervousness were averaged to obtain a measure of fear (� � .85). Resignation was assessed with a single rating.7
To estimate the relation between adherence to honor norms and emotional reactions, we first analyzed the entire set of reactions with a general linear model with the four reactions as dependent variables, type of emotion as repeated measure factor, adherence to honor norms as a continuous independent variable, and partici- pants’ age as a covariate variable. Age was included because emotional reactions may vary across age, introducing uninteresting variance. Results showed a significant main effect of type of emotion, F(3, 36) � 6.50, p � .001, p�2 � .32, which simply indicates that participants, on average, showed less joy (M � 2.53) and fear (M � 3.15) than anger (M � 4.67) and resignation (M � 4.57). More interesting, there was an interaction between type of emotion and adherence to honor norms, F(3, 36) � 10.53, p � .001, p�2 � .47, indicating that participants with different levels of adherence to honor norms showed a different pattern of emo- tional reactions (Table 1). Univariate regressions showed that participants with a strong adherence to honor norms appeared significantly more angry, t(13) � 3.61, p � .001, B � 0.91, with
Sr � .64; less fearful, t(13) � �2.24, p � .05, B � �0.45, with Sr � .54; less joyful, t(13) � �2.84, p � .025, B � �0.62, with Sr � �.54, and less resigned, t(13) � �2.38, p � .01, B � �0.48, with Sr � �.45, after being insulted than participants with a low adherence to honor norms.8 Thus, after an insult and com- pared with participants with a weak adherence to honor norms, participants with a strong adherence to honor norms displayed more facial expressions and body language indicating aggression and hostility.
Perceived Hostility
In analyzing participants’ responses to emotional faces, we first checked whether accuracy of classification of the faces was dif- ferent for different emotions, across conditions (insult vs. no insult), and across different levels of adherence to honor norms. Upon establishing no influence of the latter two variables on the accuracy level, we tested whether the perceived intensity of the emotion displayed by the picture was influenced by condition and adherence to honor norms. For the sake of completeness, we also tested perceived intensity on correctly identified faces. In these analyses, we did not include neutral faces because neither accuracy
4 Despite the small sample size, the analyses complied with all the requirements (only one factor extracted, high communalities; n � 25) that, according to Preacher and MacCallum (2002), guaranteed reliable factor structures in small samples.
5 All analyses controlled for differences between the two insulters. There was no effect of insulter on angriness, t(15) � 1.55, p � .15, nor was there an effect of insulter on resignation, t(15) � 1.00, p � .92. There was an effect, however, of insulter on fearfulness, t(15) � 4.20, p � .05, B � 0.85.
6 All emotional reactions were obtained after the bumping incident, because no comparable set opportunity existed that could be used to measure emotional reactions in the control condition.
7 Correlations for observers’ judgments were .71 for anger, .38 for fear, .38 for resignation (all ps � .05, one-tailed), and �.02 for joy (ns.). This lack of agreement for joy may be explained by the situation (being insulted), in which joy was not appropriated and in fact, showed a low mean (M � 2.50) and variance (SD � 1.24).
8 One might well be concerned with the physical condition of the participant in relation to the insult. However, there was no effect of participant’s height on angriness, t(15) � 1.00, p � .67, resignation, t(15) � 1.00, p � .78, and fearfulness, t(15) � 1.00, p � .35. There was, however, an effect of height on joyfulness, t(15) � 2.26, p � .04, B � 0.12, Sr � .50. Our subsequent analyses controlled for this effect.
Table 1 Estimated Means for Emotional Reactions for Low- and High-Honor Participants
Emotional reaction Low honor High honor tsimple tpartial
Anger 3.48 (3.62) 5.99 (5.98) 3.37*** 3.61***
Fear 3.79 (3.78) 2.61 (2.48) 1.89* 2.24**
Joy 3.52 (3.36) 1.84 (1.85) �2.61** �2.83**
Resignation 5.06 (5.27) 3.89 (3.91) �1.59 �2.37**
Note. Least square estimated means for 1 SD above (high honor) and 1 SD below (low honor) the honor scale sample mean. Values in parentheses are the estimated means for the average age. tsimple � t test and statistical significance for the univariate regression with the corresponding emotional reaction as a dependent variable. tpartial � t test and significance of the same regression with participant age effect held constant. * p � .10. ** p � .05. *** p � .001.
871BRIEF REPORTS
of classification nor perceived intensity can be interpreted for faces showing no emotion.
Emotional faces were generally identified correctly except for the fearful face, which was classified as showing surprise by half of the sample. Accuracy levels for insulted participants were 94%, 50%, and 89% for the angry, fearful, and sad faces, respectively, compared with 94%, 35%, and 94%, respectively, for not-insulted participants. We analyzed the influence of condition (insult vs. no insult) and adherence to honor norms on the accuracy (correct vs. incorrect) of classification for angry, fearful, and sad faces using a generalized linear model (Firth, 1991; Nelder & Wedderburn, 1972) and the generalized estimating equations (GEE) method (Liang & Zeger, 1986) to account for correlated responses. Essen- tially, we ran a logistic regression with accuracy as the dependent variable; adherence to honor norms, condition, and type of emo- tional face as independent variables; and type of emotional face as a repeated measure factor.9 Results showed that accuracy changed across type of emotional face. In particular, participants were less accurate in classifying fearful faces than angry (Z � 3.10, p � .002) and sad (Z � 2.11, p � .03) faces. The analysis yielded no significant effect of adherence to honor norms concerning condi- tion and its interaction with type of emotional face.
After establishing that the key variables of our study did not affect the accuracy of classification of the faces into the correct emotion category, we analyzed the perceived intensity of the emotions displayed in the picture. To study the effects of adher- ence to honor norms on perceived intensity, we used a general linear model with adherence to honor norms, condition (insult vs. no insult), and their interaction as independent variables; intensity scores as the dependent variable; and type of emotional face (angry, fearful, sad) as the within-participant factor. This model allows testing the overall effects and possible differential effects of the independent variables across different emotional faces. Both independent variables were centered before the interaction was computed (Aiken & West, 1991). The analysis yielded no statis- tically significant difference between insulted participants and not-insulted participants, F(1, 27) � 1, p � .33; no overall effect of adherence to honor norms, F(1, 27) � 1, p � .66; and no overall interaction, F(1, 27) � 3.09, p � .09. However, more importantly, the analysis demonstrated a statistically significant three-way in- teraction among condition, adherence to honor norms, and type of emotional face, F(2, 54) � 4.61, p � .01, p�2 � .15. This
interaction indicates that the interaction between adherence to honor norms and condition is different for different emotional faces. To probe the three-way interaction, we performed a regres- sion analysis with adherence to honor norms, condition (insult vs. no insult), and their interaction as independent variables for each type of emotional face. In regard to the angry face, we found a statistically significant interaction of adherence to honor norms and condition, t(27) � 2.61, p � .014, B � 0.62, with Sr � .43, and no linear effects. Simple slope analysis (Aiken & West, 1991) showed that insulted participants with a strong adherence to honor norms (1 SD above the mean) perceived the angry face as angrier than not-insulted participants with a strong adherence to honor norms, t(27) � 2.62, p � .021, B � 0.31, with Sr � .43. No statistically significant difference was found between insulted and not-insulted participants with a weak adherence to culture-of- honor norms (1 SD below the mean; Figure 1), t(27) � 1.38, p � .43. Finally, as further support of our hypothesis, there was a significant positive relation between adherence to honor norms and perception of anger in the angry face after an insult, t(27) � 2.06, p � .020, B � 0.35, with Sr � .57. No statistically significant result was found in the no-insult condition, t(27) � �1.41, p � .18. In regard to responses to fearful faces, we found no linear effects and a weak interaction between honor norms and condition, t(27) � �2.04, p � .050, with Sr � .36. Although the pattern of estimated intensity of the fearful face is suggestive (Table 2), the overall regression model was not significant, F(3, 27) � 1.58, p � .22, and a subsequent simple slope analysis showed a nonsignifi- cant effect of adherence to honor norm in both the insult condition and the no-insult condition. Accordingly, we refrain from inter- preting this result. No effect was found of the independent vari- ables on responses to a sad face.
Finally, we repeated the previous analyses of perceived intensity for each type of emotional face only for participants who perceived
9 We fit the logistic model with SAS PROC GENMOD, defining a binomial distribution for the dependent variable and a logistic link func- tion. The within-participant effect was emotional face (angry, sad, and fearful), which was modeled using a compound-symmetry covariance matrix (cf. Liang & Zeger, 1986). Because of the presence of many zeros in the complete cross-tabulation of the independent and dependent vari- ables, we could not fit the full model. We fit a submodel with all the main effects and the interaction between emotional face and condition (insult vs. no insult).
Figure 1. Mean perceived intensity of an angry face for 1 SD above (high honor) and 1 SD below (low honor) the honor scale sample mean at the two levels of experimental manipulation (insult vs. no insult).
Table 2 Perceived Intensity of Faces as Presented and Correctly Classified
Low honor High honor
Emotional face No insult Insult No insult Insult
Angry 3.41 (3.42) 3.03 (2.40) 2.41 (3.10) 3.80 (3.60) Sad 2.99 (2.94) 3.55 (3.34) 3.40 (3.20) 3.32 (3.42) Fearful 3.33 (4.12) 2.58 (1.40) 2.17 (2.55) 3.02 (2.38)
Note. Least square estimated means for 1 SD above (high honor) and 1 SD below (low honor) the honor scale sample mean at the two levels of experimental manipulation (insult vs. no insult). Values in parentheses are the estimated means only for participants who correctly classified the face.
872 BRIEF REPORTS
the face as displaying the correct emotion.10 In regard to the angry face, a regression with adherence of honor norms, condition (insult vs. no insult), and their interaction as independent variables was conducted on the 30 participants who perceived the face as dis- playing anger. Coherent with previous analyses, the model yielded a significant interaction between adherence to honor norms and condition, t(26) � 2.09, p � .046, B � 0.52, with Sr � .34, and no linear effect. No significant effects were found for perceived intensity of correctly classified sad faces and fearful faces.
Discussion
The present research investigated the relations between adher- ence to honor norms and emotions. Using a field experiment among train travelers, we examined the effect of an insult and adherence to honor norms on people’s immediate emotional reac- tions and on their perception of hostility in subsequent stimuli.
Our research is the first to show a direct relation among insult, adherence to honor norms, and immediate emotional reactions, thereby extending previous research in important ways. First, the present results show that, after an insult, individuals with a strong adherence to honor norms differed greatly in their emotional reactions from individuals with a weak adherence to honor norms. The former individuals were rated by observers as angrier but less fearful, less joyful, and less resigned than the latter. In previous research, Cohen et al. (1996) obtained inconsistent findings con- cerning observed emotional reactions of insulted Southerners and Northerners, leading them to comment that “the results of Exper- iment 1 regarding anger and amusement must be treated with caution until subsequent research replicates the findings of Exper- iment 1 in ecologically natural circumstances” (p. 951). The present findings provide an elegant replication and extension of Cohen et al.’s findings in a real-world context.
On the basis of Cohen et al.’s (1996) theory, it is expected that individuals with a strong adherence to honor norms would be less resigned and less fearful after an insult than those with a weak adherence to honor norms. Our present findings are the first to provide empirical support for this hypothesized relation between adherence to honor norms and the experience of fear and resignation.
Previous research found significant differences in emotional reactions after an affront between men reared in the southern versus the northern United States. Cohen et al. (1996) suggested that these findings are traceable to differences in the strength of adherence to honor norms, because the northern and southern United States differ significantly in ascribing to these norms. However, their findings were not supported by a direct assessment of the strength of adherence to honor norms, thereby precluding the establishment of a direct relation among insult, adherence to honor norms, and emotional reactions. By incorporating a direct measure of strength of adherence to honor norms in our field experiment, we were able to establish this direct relation and demonstrate that stronger adherence to honor norms leads to more aggressive and hostile responses after an affront.
Furthermore, our present findings are the first to show that insulted individuals with a strong adherence to honor norms per- ceive more hostility in subsequent stimuli than not-insulted indi- viduals with a strong adherence to honor norms. The former were more likely to perceive angry faces as angrier than the latter. The finding that this difference in perceived hostility was only found
for angry faces suggests that individuals with a strong adherence to honor norms do not perceive neutral stimuli as more hostile or perceive more hostility in all stimuli, but they only perceive more hostility in those stimuli that involve an affront or challenge. This can be related to findings of Cohen et al. (1996), who found that an insult did not create a generalized hostility but rather hostility toward specific situations that concerned issues of honor.
Why do people with a strong adherence to honor norms react more hostile after they have been insulted? The most promising answer to this question may be derived from appraisal theories (Frijda, 1986; Roseman, 1984; Scherer, 1984; C. A. Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). According to the universal contingencies hy- pothesis, people from different cultures will experience similar emotions if they appraise a situation in the same way, whereas they will experience different emotions if they appraise a situation differently (Ellsworth, 1994; Scherer, 1997). This hypothesis im- plies that cultural differences in the emotional experiences may be explained by differences in appraisals. Cohen et al.’s (1996) find- ings regarding the different emotional reactions of individuals from honor and nonhonor cultures may be attributed to differences in their appraisals concerning the “bumping incident.” The present findings contribute to previous research and to the universal con- tingencies hypothesis by suggesting that individuals with a strong adherence to honor norms may perceive the situation as more unexpected, more unpleasant, more obstructing their goals, or more unfair. Evaluating the situation more strongly in terms of these appraisals is likely to amplify anger and hostility (see, e.g., Roseman, Wiest, & Swartz, 1994; Scherer, 1997).
Our present findings concerning both immediate emotional re- actions and subsequent perception of hostility may also be ex- plained by an argument made for cultural salience; that is, some type of events or stimuli may be seen as especially relevant in some cultures but not others (cf. Mesquita & Ellswo
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.