Total of 2 page or more There are three readings that need to be read, please write the summary and analysis according to the
Total of 2 page or more
There are three readings that need to be read, please write the summary and analysis according to the following requirement for each reading. Please do them separately by indicating 1)-3) for each question.
- A brief summary of the key argument, problem, or issue
- Suggesting the significance of the piece (how it contributes to our understanding of this topic within our class’s broad study of human information interaction)
- Posing one or more questions that you would like to probe about this reading or any other combination of strategies to get the group discussion going
Please also write a brief summary of what the readings are about for each of them, so I could know what the readings are talking about. Thank you!
Fidel. R. (2012) Five search strategies. In Human Information interaction: An ecological approach to information behaviour (pp. 97-118). The MIT Press.
Freund, L., O’Brien, H.L., Kopak, R. (2014). Getting the big picture: Supporting comprehension and learning in search [Searching as learning workshop]. Information Interaction in Context Conference. http://www.diigubc.ca/IIIXSAL/Papers/FreundObrienKopak.pdf
Freund, L., Kopak, R. & O’Brien, H.L. (2016). The effects of textual environment on reading comprehension: implications for searching as learning. The Journal of Information Science, 42(1), 79-93.
Human Information Interaction Fidel, Raya
Published by The MIT Press
Fidel, Raya. Human Information Interaction: An Ecological Approach to Information Behavior. The MIT Press, 2012. Project MUSE. muse.jhu.edu/book/21630. https://muse.jhu.edu/.
For additional information about this book
[ Access provided at 29 Jan 2022 01:05 GMT from The University of British Columbia Library ]
https://muse.jhu.edu/book/21630
5 Five Search Strategies
The concept search strategy has been part of the vocabulary of human information
behavior (HIB) since the earliest user studies. However, researchers only began to
investigate search strategies after the development of digital technology, when the
concept became a popular focus of study with the introduction of the World Wide
Web. Unlike information need , which is relatively stable, 1 search strategy addresses the
dynamic part of the search process itself. While an information need triggers a search
process, search strategies reflect the activities during the search. In addition, strategies
are considered to possess a great advantage as an object of study: While they are purely
cognitive in nature, they are observable because their use — that is, the activities during
a search — can be observed. 2 New research techniques that have been afforded by
digital technology made it possible to investigate the search process itself, and thus
its strategies.
Because the concept search strategy is relatively concrete and observable, its defini-
tion has not raised much discussion, but researchers have attributed to it a range of
interpretations and definitions and have often overlooked the need to provide their
construal even when search strategies were the focus of their studies. This chapter
briefly provides a few examples of some of these definitions and proposes a view on
search strategies that is relevant to the design of information systems.
5.1 What Is a Search Strategy?
Research into search strategies has been carried out since the late 1970s, but the inter-
pretation of the concept search strategies has been highly fluid, and even today the
concept is imbued with a plurality of meanings. HIB researchers have applied the term
to signify any aspect of an information search process that lacks its own name. Most
empirical researchers have also neglected to explain their understanding of the concept.
98 Chapter 5
In some cases, the investigators ’ construal can be inferred from the specific search
strategy they investigated. Only a few researchers provided explicit definitions for
search strategies , and some others borrowed these definitions for their own studies.
5.1.1 Implicit Construal of Search Strategy
Examples of search strategies that have been discovered in web searching without the
support of an explicit definition of the concept show that most address specific actions
in a search process and are highly concrete, mechanical, and concerned with observ-
able actions. Only a few implicit definitions enjoy some level of abstraction. Some
researchers were inconsistent in the level of abstraction of the search strategies they
investigated, identifying them along a range from highly concrete to the abstract.
The series of studies that Nigel Ford and his colleagues conducted is a typical
example of a concrete and actions-based interpretation of the concept. Ford began his
investigation of search strategies during the early period of bibliographical databases
(e.g., Ford, Wood, and Walsh 1994). Examining his research reports, it seems that he
understood search strategies to be the types of actions a searcher take to transform a
query. A recent article about the use of search strategies provided 18 strategies (Ford,
Eaglestone, and Madden 2009), such as page down , remove Boolean operators , include
quotation marks , reuse part of a query , and change operators only . Other researchers — such
as Martzoukou (2008) and Iivonen and White (2001) — recognized search strategies on
the same level of abstraction, identifying, for example, use Boolean operators or use
subject directory .
The concrete level of the search strategies ’ construal limits the range of their appli-
cability because they are to a large degree determined by the technology being used.
Search strategies that can be employed in best-match systems, 3 for instance, are dif-
ferent from those in systems with ranked output. 4 Moreover, some of the search strate-
gies that were identified are based on specific technical attributes of the search system,
such as the query language (e.g., include quotation marks , use subject directory ) and query
operators (e.g., use Boolean operators ). As a result, the strategies that were discovered
are pertinent to searches under the conditions in which they were discovered, but
they may not be applicable to other modes of information searching, such as browsing
the library shelves or asking a person for driving directions. The more abstract the
level of definition, the more modes of searching it represents.
A few scholars interpreted search strategies on a somewhat abstract level. An
example of such approach is the study by Ramirez et al. (2002) which examined the
role of computers in mediating human-to-human communication, that is, informa-
tion-seeking when the source of information is another human. It seems that they
Five Search Strategies 99
understood search strategies to be the relationship between the information seeker
(the communicator) and the object of the information acquired (the target). They
distinguished three main types of strategies (Ramirez et al. 2002, 219 – 221):
• Interactive strategies entail direct interaction between communicator and target
during which different tactics are enabled to elicit desired information; for example,
the communicator interrogates the target, discloses information designed to elicit
reciprocal disclosure, and attempts to relax the target in order to acquire
information.
• Active strategies involve acquiring information from other individuals but without
direct interaction with the target, as is the case, for example, with the use of third-
party information sources, such as acquiring information through email exchanges
and chats with others familiar with the target.
• Passive strategies involve acquiring information about a target through unobtrusive
observation, such as being “ carbon copied ” on messages, eavesdropping on a conversa-
tion, or lurking on a listserv.
Ramirez et al. ’ s classification demonstrates that universal, or abstract, construal of
search strategies makes them independent of the technology used, and certainly free
of association with technical attributes of an information system, whether a human
or a machine.
In summary, the unsystematic nature of the use of the concept search strategy , sup-
ported by the lack of explicit understanding of the concept, created a muddled trail
of research about search strategies in which only the term itself is common to all
investigations.
5.1.2 Definitions of Search Strategy
Most explicit definitions of search strategies were universal and abstract in nature. The
most universal one was offered by Belkin and his colleagues (Belkin, Marchetti, and
Cool 1993; Belkin et al. 1995). They defined search strategies as the behaviors in which
people engage when searching for information. One might claim that this definition
is too general and actually represents the more general concept information-seeking
behavior (ISB), thus making it difficult to differentiate between the two concepts. Nev-
ertheless, using this approach, they presented four mutually exclusive dimensions (or
facets) of strategies that together create search strategies. That is, each search strategy
is a combination of elements drawn from the four facets. Each facet, in turn, includes
a continuum of elements that Belkin et al. (1993) derived from informal analysis of
empirical studies. For each dimension they listed the two extreme strategies. 5 The
100 Chapter 5
dimensions were method of interaction (from scanning to searching); goal of interac-
tion (from learning to selecting); mode of retrieval (from recognition to specification);
and resources considered (from information to metainformation). 6 These dimensions
demonstrate a very broad construal of search strategies , and raise some questions. It is
difficult to accept goal of interaction , for example, as a dimension of a strategy. A goal
may provide a reason for selecting a certain search strategy but it is not a dimension
of it. This is because strategies are usually associated with activities, whereas goals do
not represent activities and are not even directly identified by them, since various
activities may lead to the same goal and one activity may lead to the accomplishment
of more than one goal. In addition, this broad definition cannot guide researchers in
discovering other strategies, and thus limits the possible strategies to those Belkin
et al. have defined.
A definition that is universal, yet in sync with the notion of strategy in everyday
language, and the first one formulated in HIB, was offered by Marcia Bates (1981). She
explained that a search strategy is: “ An approach to or plan for a whole search. A
search strategy is used to inform or to determine specific search formulation decisions;
it operates at a level above term choice and command use ” (142). This definition is
not bounded by dimensions or technology, and places search strategies as a compo-
nent of information-seeking behavior. An example of a strategy might be: First I ’ ll try
a couple of terms, and if I don ’ t get good results, I ’ ll look for better terms either by
browsing the results or by thinking about the problem in light of what was retrieved.
Gary Marchionini (1995) construed search strategies in a similar way and also placed
the concept in an abstraction hierarchy of concepts in searching behavior, in which
each level is affected by the level above it. Marchionini ’ s hierarchy moves from the
concrete to the abstract:
• “ Moves are finely grained actions manifested as discrete behavioral actions such as
walking to a shelf, picking up a book, pressing a key, clicking a mouse, or touching
an item from a menu ” (74).
• “ Tactics are discrete intellectual choices or prompts manifested as behavioral actions
during an information-seeking session … for example, when restricting the search to
a specific field or document type in order to narrow the search results ” (74).
• “ A Strategy is the approach that an information seeker takes to a problem. Strategies
are those sets of ordered tactics that are consciously selected, applied, and monitored
to solve an information problem ” (72).
• “ Patterns are sometimes conscious but most often reflect internalized behaviors that
can be discerned over time and across different information problems and searches.
Five Search Strategies 101
Patterns may be caused by chunked strategies or tactics that people internalize though
repetition and experience ” (72). One manifestation of patterns is, for example, an
individual ’ s searching style .
Iris Xie (2007) created a similar hierarchy with an understanding of search strategies
that was more general than the previous definitions, and included the goals of a
search. She explained:
Information-seeking strategies comprise interactive intentions and retrieval tactics . Interactive inten-
tions refer to subgoals that a user has to achieve in the process of accomplishing his or her current
search goal/search task. … Retrieval tactics are represented by methods and entities with attributes.
Methods refer to the techniques users apply to interact with data/information, knowledge,
concept/term, format, item/objects/site, process/status, location, system and humans. (Xie 2007,
emphasis added)
These definitions have had an impact on other studies. Vakkari (1999), for example,
used Belkin et al. ’ s (1993) dimensions among other constructs when he analyzed how
an information problem ’ s structure (i.e., structured versus ill-structured) affects search
strategies, and Xie ’ s (2007) definitions were inspired by the approaches of Belkin
et al., Bates, and Marchionini in addition to other views. The definitions have guided
empirical studies as well. Thatcher (2006), for example, employed Marchionini ’ s hier-
archy when he investigated the search strategies that were employed by 80 study
participants. He identified 12 strategies, which he named “ cognitive search strategies, ”
including the following:
The participant went to a search engine that was known to them [ sic ]; participants used different
search engines to conduct the same search; the participant deliberately opened multiple browser
windows to conduct different searches simultaneously; the participant relied solely on hyperlinks
from the homepage to get from one webpage to another. (Thatcher 2006, 1059-1063)
Thatcher ’ s search strategies are different in nature and level of abstraction from
those identified by Marchionini, who envisioned them to be laid out on a spectrum
with opposite ends: the analytical and the browsing strategies. The analytical strategies
are “ planned, goal driven, deterministic, formal, and discrete, ” while the browsing
strategies are “ opportunistic, data driven, heuristic, informal, and continuous ”
(Marchionini 1995, 73). 7 While widely accepted (if not always correctly), the distinc-
tion between these two types of strategies is not compatible with the approach pre-
sented in this book. According to the view presented here, each search is driven by a
goal (to solve an information problem) rather than by data, regardless of the strategies
employed. In addition, every strategy is a plan. Thus, even a decision to start a search
without a specific plan (i.e., browsing) is a plan. With these conceptions, Marchionini ’ s
102 Chapter 5
definitions represent attributes of searching and surfing (see section 2.1.1.1). Since these
are two modes of acquiring information, they are dichotomous, rather than the oppo-
site ends of a spectrum.
In conclusion, definitions of search strategies are usually universal and abstract and
can guide other researchers in identifying specific strategies, whether on a conceptual
level or in empirical studies. But these definitions have had one drawback: Using them
has generated an unruly repertoire of strategies in which each researcher has employed
her own view on how to carve out strategies from an analysis of the literature or from
the data at hand. In addition, the number of search strategies is growing constantly
as new ones are discovered, usually without attempting to place them in relation to
other strategies. Most concerning is the diversity in the levels of abstraction of the
search strategies that have been generated, which ranged from the physical actions to
plans of action. 8 This inconsistency points to fundamental differences among the
interpretations of the concept. With the continually increasing number of strategies,
it is useful to find a configuration that may contain them. One promising approach
to reduce this confusion is to view a search strategy as a category of plans, general
approaches, or interactive intentions (see section 5.4).
5.2 The Conditions That Shape the Use of a Strategy
Various studies identified the conditions that shape the use of a strategy, which are
usually termed “ factors affecting the choice of search strategies. ” Some of the findings
of these studies were based on an analysis of previous studies (e.g., Vakkari 1999), and
others on empirical research (e.g., Ford, Eaglestone, and Madden 2009; Rouet 2003).
In a typical investigation the researcher selects a factor of interest and analyzes or tests
its effect. Thus, Vakkari (1999) examined the effect of the structure of the information
problem; Ford et al. (2009) looked at individual differences; and Rouet (2003) tested
the effect of task specificity and prior knowledge.
Studies of this type face various challenges. For example, the definitions that
researchers employed were unable to lead investigators to the variables that are likely
to affect the selection of search strategies. It is difficult to think about a variable that
may affect, say, the strategy “ using quotation marks ” — except for the obvious one:
whether or not a searcher is familiar with the strategy. With these definitions, research-
ers have had to use a trial-and-error approach when they select the variables to be
tested. Another challenge is the relatively large number of search strategies that were
defined by researchers. Thus, even if investigators find a variable that may affect one
strategy, the variable may leave the rest of the search strategies unaffected. Indeed,
Five Search Strategies 103
typical findings of such studies that tested an array of search strategies pointed to one
or two strategies that were affected by the tested variables but found no factors that
affected the other strategies. This way, one can state that an actor with high value on
variable X is more likely to employ category A than an actor with low values, but the
question “ Which search strategies are an actor with low value is likely to select? ”
remains unanswered. 9 Considering search strategies as a category overcomes these and
other challenges (see section 5.4.2).
5.3 Systems Designed to Support Strategies
Regardless of the definition of search strategies , most scholars agree that information
systems that support the strategies are better than those that ignore them. Yet only a
few researchers have provided systems requirements to support the strategies they
unveiled or redefined. Most systematic among these researchers were Belkin, Mar-
chetti, and Cool (1993). They methodically analyzed each strategy they had defined
to identify the problems that one may encounter when employing it. Thinking about
ways a system could alleviate the problems they identified, they generated 36 require-
ments for information systems interfaces (see section 10.3.3.1). They recommended,
for example, that a system provide a “ display of resources with explanations of link
type, ” “ direct retrieval of example information items from selected terms, ” and “ struc-
tured representation of query and search ” (Belkin et al. 1993, 330 – 331).
While Belkin et al. (1993) offered highly specific requirements, based on all the
search strategies they had identified, Bates (2007) focused on one search strategy —
browsing — and offered a much more general interface requirement. She explained that
“ [g]ood browsable interfaces would consist of rich scenes, full of potential objects of
interest, that the eye can take in at once ( massively parallel processing ), then select items
within the scene to give closer attention to. ” She also presented a model of such an
interface that was developed by Toms (2000) as an example of a good interface. 10
Both Belkin et al. (1993) and Bates (2007) offered implications for the design of
universal systems, regardless of the characteristics of the searchers. Another approach
is to focus on the searchers, identifying the strategy that would be useful to them, and
then generate design requirements based on the actors ’ information behavior. It is
unrealistic to design search systems for each individual, but it is reasonable to do so
for a particular community of actors. In this case an analyst may ask, What strategies
will play a central role in these actors ’ search for information? Once this question is
answered, implications for design could also be based on the typical characteristics of
the actors. Browsing support provided to scientists, for instance, should probably be
104 Chapter 5
different from that offered to youth looking for health information. This difference is
required not only due to the dissimilarity in the actors ’ cognitive resources and
context, but also due to the centrality of the browsing strategy for each community.
While browsing is likely to be essential to youth looking for information in an unfa-
miliar area, scientists are not likely to employ it as a central strategy. Section 5.4.3
provides a comparison between two communities ’ strategy selections and the resulting
design requirements as an example.
5.4 Search Strategy as a Category
A search strategy is cognitive in nature — because plans, general approaches, or interac-
tive intentions are all hatched in the human mind — regardless of the contextual situ-
ation that shapes it. In my work I have applied the conceptual framework cognitive
work analysis (CWA) to HIB (see chapters 11 and 12). CWA views strategies in associa-
tion with decision-making processes (see section 11.1). Vicente (1999) — based on
Rasmussen (1981) — defined a strategy as “ a category of cognitive task procedures that
transform an initial state of knowledge into a final state of knowledge ” (220).
Rasmussen, Pejtersen, and Goodstein (1994) explained that cognitive processes
within the same category — that is, the same strategy — “ share important characteristics,
such as a particular kind of mental model, a certain mode of interpretation of the
observed evidence, and a coherent set of tactical planning rules ” (70). 11 Vicente (1999)
further explained that each strategy is “ based on a different set of performance criteria,
and requires a different kind of information support ” (219).
Strategies can serve various decision processes, such as diagnosis, evaluation, or
planning (Rasmussen et al. 1994).
5.4.1 Five Search Strategies
In the area of information science, field studies in information retrieval (IR) that were
guided by CWA have defined strategies that are employed in the information search
process. 12 More specifically, Pejtersen (1984) uncovered five distinct search strategies
(Pejtersen 1979) in her study of fiction retrieval in public libraries. Later studies have
observed the use of these strategies and found no additional ones. 13 Browsing and
analytical strategies are included in this set, but their definitions are different from
Marchionini ’ s (1995). The strategies are presented in table 5.1
Although each search strategy is derived from a certain mental model, actors may
switch strategy in the middle of a search. 14 One may use a library catalog employing
the analytical strategy, for instance, to find the location of a book on a particular topic,
Five Search Strategies 105
but browse the shelf for additional sources once that book has been located. Similarly,
an actor may enter a complex search query but continue browsing through links when
the results are not satisfactory. When conducting a study of searching behavior, it is
sometimes difficult to detect a strategy shift. This difficulty is particularly the case
when the analysis is based only on observation or on transaction logs. In fact, it is
very difficult to identify search strategies without access to the cognitive processes
involved in the specific search. A transaction log of a web search may show, for
example, two terms in the search box followed by many clicks on links. Without
understanding the mental model the actor had, it is impossible to determine if he
employed the browsing or the analytical strategy. An awareness of the cognitive pro-
cesses is required for the definition of search strategies because they reflect a mental
model rather than specific procedures. Observation and analyses of transaction logs
by themselves can identify only procedures and cannot provide insight to the mental
model that is employed in a search.
5.4.1.1 The Browsing Strategy
The browsing strategy (intuitive scanning following leads by association without much
planning ahead ) had been identified long before computers began to be used for infor-
mation retrieval. Although its most commonly recognized manifestation has been
browsing bookshelves, the introduction of hypertext made browsing a highly viable
strategy when searching digital information systems. A person who decides to browse
in order to find information for making a decision might think: “ Let me start here
and see where it takes me. ” When searching the web, one might follow this decision
by clicking on links or using a directory.
Table 5.1 Search strategies and their definitions
Search strategy Definition
Browsing Intuitive scanning following leads by association without much planning ahead
Analytical Explicit consideration of attributes of the information problem and of the search system
Empirical Based on previous experience, using rules and tactics that were successful in the past
Known site Going directly to the place where the information is located
Similarity Finding information based on a previous example that is similar to the current need
106 Chapter 5
This view of browsing is different from Marchionini ’ s (1995, 73) not only in meaning
but also in type (he argued that browsing strategies are “ opportunistic, data driven,
heuristic, informal, and continuous ” ). His interpretation of the strategy is based on
the category “ elements that drive a search ” (opportunistic, data driven) and on the
category “ manner in which the search progresses ” (heuristic, informal, continuous).
That is, while all these elements that define browsing are cognitive, they belong
to different categories. In fact, according to the CWA definition, a browsing strategy
can fit in Marchionini ’ s analytical one because it can be goal driven, deterministic,
and formal.
The browsing strategy has attracted more research interest than any other strategy,
and has had the widest range of interpretations (see reviews of these in Chang
and Rice 1993 and in Rice, McCreadie, and Chang 2001). One example of a
thorough conceptual investigation into the concept is Bates ’ s (2007) question:
“ What is browsing — really? ” She placed the concept in human development and
found that “ most animals have a propensity toward exploratory behaviour. ” Viewing
browsing in the context of this behavior led her to conclude that “ browsing is a cogni-
tive and behavioural expression of this exploratory behaviour, ” and that in humans,
curiosity is “ the in-built motivation for this exploratory behaviour. ” Thus, her defini-
tion is:
Browsing is the activity of engaging in a series of glimpses, each of which exposes the browser
to objects of potential interest; depending on interest, the browser may or may not examine
more closely one or more of the (physical or represented) objects; this examination, depending
on interest, may or may not lead the browser to (physically or conceptually) acquire the object.
(Bates 2007) 15
On the empirical research front, Shan-Ju L. Chang (2005) carried out the most
comprehensive series of studies on browsing. Besides identifying the dimensions that
can support a description of browsing, 16 she created a multidimensional framework
for understanding the influences on the process as well as the consequences of
browsing.
In addition to being the most explored strategy, browsing is also the most perva-
sively used strategy in information searching. While it is a strategy on its own, it can
also occur as a sequence when other strategies are employed. Retrieving a desired book
from the library shelves, for example, requires some browsing on the shelf before the
specific book can be located. Similarly, when one finds a web site, using any search
strategy, that provides the needed information, one might click on additional links
for further exploration. Despite its prevalence, no formal training about how to browse
Five Search Strategies 107
exists (to my knowledge), 17 and search engines provide no support for the strategy, 18
as evidenced by the common lost-in-cyberspace situation.
5.4.1.2 The Analytical Strategy
Using the analytical strategy, one explores the information need on the one hand
and systems capabilities on the other. 19 The next step is to match the need and
the system ’ s attributes — or, translate the need into a query
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.
