Contributions of Neuroscience to Our Understanding of Cognitive Development Dont! The Secret of Self-Control Provide an in-d
Choose one of the following articles: that is attached
- Contributions of Neuroscience to Our Understanding of Cognitive Development
- Don’t! The Secret of Self-Control
Provide an in-depth analysis of the selected article and respond to the following:
- Write a summary.
- Describe the main points of the article and how it relates to the week’s course and text readings.
- Evaluate the article on the basis of your own thoughts and perspectives on the topic covered.
Submission Details:
- Support your responses with examples.
- Cite any sources in APA format.
DON'T! Lehrer, Jonah . The New Yorker ; New York Vol. 85, Iss. 14, (May 18, 2009): 26.
ProQuest document link
ABSTRACT Columbia psychology professor Walter Mischel is profiled. In the 1960's, Mischel and colleagues performed an
experiment designed to explore why some children are able to delay gratification and others can't. Now subjects
from that experiment are being asked to return for functional MRI studies, in the hope that the neurology of self-
control may be mapped. FULL TEXT
In the late nineteen-sixties, Carolyn Weisz, a four-year-old with long brown hair, was invited into a "game room" at
the Bing Nursery School, on the campus of Stanford University. The room was little more than a large closet,
containing a desk and a chair. Carolyn was asked to sit down in the chair and pick a treat from a tray of
marshmallows, cookies, and pretzel sticks. Carolyn chose the marshmallow. Although she's now forty-four, Carolyn
still has a weakness for those air-puffed balls of corn syrup and gelatine. "I know I shouldn't like them," she says.
"But they're just so delicious!" A researcher then made Carolyn an offer: she could either eat one marshmallow
right away or, if she was willing to wait while he stepped out for a few minutes, she could have two marshmallows
when he returned. He said that if she rang a bell on the desk while he was away he would come running back, and
she could eat one marshmallow but would forfeit the second. Then he left the room.
Although Carolyn has no direct memory of the experiment, and the scientists would not release any information
about the subjects, she strongly suspects that she was able to delay gratification. "I've always been really good at
waiting," Carolyn told me. "If you give me a challenge or a task, then I'm going to find a way to do it, even if it means
not eating my favorite food." Her mother, Karen Sortino, is still more certain: "Even as a young kid, Carolyn was very
patient. I'm sure she would have waited." But her brother Craig, who also took part in the experiment, displayed less
fortitude. Craig, a year older than Carolyn, still remembers the torment of trying to wait. "At a certain point, it must
have occurred to me that I was all by myself," he recalls. "And so I just started taking all the candy." According to
Craig, he was also tested with little plastic toys–he could have a second one if he held out–and he broke into the
desk, where he figured there would be additional toys. "I took everything I could," he says. "I cleaned them out.
After that, I noticed the teachers encouraged me to not go into the experiment room anymore."
Footage of these experiments, which were conducted over several years, is poignant, as the kids struggle to delay
gratification for just a little bit longer. Some cover their eyes with their hands or turn around so that they can't see
the tray. Others start kicking the desk, or tug on their pigtails, or stroke the marshmallow as if it were a tiny stuffed
animal. One child, a boy with neatly parted hair, looks carefully around the room to make sure that nobody can see
him. Then he picks up an Oreo, delicately twists it apart, and licks off the white cream filling before returning the
cookie to the tray, a satisfied look on his face.
Most of the children were like Craig. They struggled to resist the treat and held out for an average of less than
three minutes. "A few kids ate the marshmallow right away," Walter Mischel, the Stanford professor of psychology
in charge of the experiment, remembers. "They didn't even bother ringing the bell. Other kids would stare directly at
the marshmallow and then ring the bell thirty seconds later." About thirty per cent of the children, however, were
like Carolyn. They successfully delayed gratification until the researcher returned, some fifteen minutes later.
These kids wrestled with temptation but found a way to resist.
The initial goal of the experiment was to identify the mental processes that allowed some people to delay
gratification while others simply surrendered. After publishing a few papers on the Bing studies in the early
seventies, Mischel moved on to other areas of personality research. "There are only so many things you can do
with kids trying not to eat marshmallows."
But occasionally Mischel would ask his three daughters, all of whom attended the Bing, about their friends from
nursery school. "It was really just idle dinnertime conversation," he says. "I'd ask them, 'How's Jane? How's Eric?
How are they doing in school?' " Mischel began to notice a link between the children's academic performance as
teen-agers and their ability to wait for the second marshmallow. He asked his daughters to assess their friends
academically on a scale of zero to five. Comparing these ratings with the original data set, he saw a correlation.
"That's when I realized I had to do this seriously," he says. Starting in 1981, Mischel sent out a questionnaire to all
the reachable parents, teachers, and academic advisers of the six hundred and fifty-three subjects who had
participated in the marshmallow task, who were by then in high school. He asked about every trait he could think
of, from their capacity to plan and think ahead to their ability to "cope well with problems" and get along with their
peers. He also requested their S.A.T. scores.
Once Mischel began analyzing the results, he noticed that low delayers, the children who rang the bell quickly,
seemed more likely to have behavioral problems, both in school and at home. They got lower S.A.T. scores. They
struggled in stressful situations, often had trouble paying attention, and found it difficult to maintain friendships.
The child who could wait fifteen minutes had an S.A.T. score that was, on average, two hundred and ten points
higher than that of the kid who could wait only thirty seconds.
Carolyn Weisz is a textbook example of a high delayer. She attended Stanford as an undergraduate, and got her
Ph.D. in social psychology at Princeton. She's now an associate psychology professor at the University of Puget
Sound. Craig, meanwhile, moved to Los Angeles and has spent his career doing "all kinds of things" in the
entertainment industry, mostly in production. He's currently helping to write and produce a film. "Sure, I wish I had
been a more patient person," Craig says. "Looking back, there are definitely moments when it would have helped
me make better career choices and stuff."
Mischel and his colleagues continued to track the subjects into their late thirties–Ozlem Ayduk, an assistant
professor of psychology at the University of California at Berkeley, found that low-delaying adults have a
significantly higher body-mass index and are more likely to have had problems with drugs–but it was frustrating to
have to rely on self-reports. "There's often a gap between what people are willing to tell you and how they behave in
the real world," he explains. And so, last year, Mischel, who is now a professor at Columbia, and a team of
collaborators began asking the original Bing subjects to travel to Stanford for a few days of experiments in an fMRI
machine. Carolyn says she will be participating in the scanning experiments later this summer; Craig completed a
survey several years ago, but has yet to be invited to Palo Alto. The scientists are hoping to identify the particular
brain regions that allow some people to delay gratification and control their temper. They're also conducting a
variety of genetic tests, as they search for the hereditary characteristics that influence the ability to wait for a
second marshmallow.
If Mischel and his team succeed, they will have outlined the neural circuitry of self-control. For decades,
psychologists have focussed on raw intelligence as the most important variable when it comes to predicting
success in life. Mischel argues that intelligence is largely at the mercy of self-control: even the smartest kids still
need to do their homework. "What we're really measuring with the marshmallows isn't will power or self-control,"
Mischel says. "It's much more important than that. This task forces kids to find a way to make the situation work
for them. They want the second marshmallow, but how can they get it? We can't control the world, but we can
control how we think about it."
Walter Mischel is a slight, elegant man with a shaved head and a face of deep creases. He talks with a Brooklyn
bluster and he tends to act out his sentences, so that when he describes the marshmallow task he takes on the
body language of an impatient four-year-old. "If you want to know why some kids can wait and others can't, then
you've got to think like they think," Mischel says.
Mischel was born in Vienna, in 1930. His father was a modestly successful businessman with a fondness for cafe
society and Esperanto, while his mother spent many of her days lying on the couch with an ice pack on her
forehead, trying to soothe her frail nerves. The family considered itself fully assimilated, but after the Nazi
annexation of Austria, in 1938, Mischel remembers being taunted in school by the Hitler Youth and watching as his
father, hobbled by childhood polio, was forced to limp through the streets in his pajamas. A few weeks after the
takeover, while the family was burning evidence of their Jewish ancestry in the fireplace, Walter found a long-
forgotten certificate of U.S. citizenship issued to his maternal grandfather decades earlier, thus saving his family.
The family settled in Brooklyn, where Mischel's parents opened up a five-and-dime. Mischel attended New York
University, studying poetry under Delmore Schwartz and Allen Tate, and taking studio-art classes with Philip
Guston. He also became fascinated by psychoanalysis and new measures of personality, such as the Rorschach
test. "At the time, it seemed like a mental X-ray machine," he says. "You could solve a person by showing them a
picture." Although he was pressured to join his uncle's umbrella business, he ended up pursuing a Ph.D. in clinical
psychology at Ohio State.
But Mischel noticed that academic theories had limited application, and he was struck by the futility of most
personality science. He still flinches at the naivete of graduate students who based their diagnoses on a battery of
meaningless tests. In 1955, Mischel was offered an opportunity to study the "spirit possession" ceremonies of the
Orisha faith in Trinidad, and he leapt at the chance. Although his research was supposed to involve the use of
Rorschach tests to explore the connections between the unconscious and the behavior of people when possessed,
Mischel soon grew interested in a different project. He lived in a part of the island that was evenly split between
people of East Indian and of African descent; he noticed that each group defined the other in broad stereotypes.
"The East Indians would describe the Africans as impulsive hedonists, who were always living for the moment and
never thought about the future," he says. "The Africans, meanwhile, would say that the East Indians didn't know
how to live and would stuff money in their mattress and never enjoy themselves."
Mischel took young children from both ethnic groups and offered them a simple choice: they could have a
miniature chocolate bar right away or, if they waited a few days, they could get a much bigger chocolate bar.
Mischel's results failed to justify the stereotypes–other variables, such as whether or not the children lived with
their father, turned out to be much more important–but they did get him interested in the question of delayed
gratification. Why did some children wait and not others? What made waiting possible? Unlike the broad traits
supposedly assessed by personality tests, self-control struck Mischel as potentially measurable.
In 1958, Mischel became an assistant professor in the Department of Social Relations at Harvard. One of his first
tasks was to develop a survey course on "personality assessment," but Mischel quickly concluded that, while
prevailing theories held personality traits to be broadly consistent, the available data didn't back up this
assumption. Personality, at least as it was then conceived, couldn't be reliably assessed at all. A few years later, he
was hired as a consultant on a personality assessment initiated by the Peace Corps. Early Peace Corps volunteers
had sparked several embarrassing international incidents–one mailed a postcard on which she expressed disgust
at the sanitary habits of her host country–so the Kennedy Administration wanted a screening process to eliminate
people unsuited for foreign assignments. Volunteers were tested for standard personality traits, and Mischel
compared the results with ratings of how well the volunteers performed in the field. He found no correlation; the
time-consuming tests predicted nothing. At this point, Mischel realized that the problem wasn't the tests–it was
their premise. Psychologists had spent decades searching for traits that exist independently of circumstance, but
what if personality can't be separated from context? "It went against the way we'd been thinking about personality
since the four humors and the ancient Greeks," he says.
While Mischel was beginning to dismantle the methods of his field, the Harvard psychology department was in
tumult. In 1960, the personality psychologist Timothy Leary helped start the Harvard Psilocybin Project, which
consisted mostly of self-experimentation. Mischel remembers graduate students' desks giving way to mattresses,
and large packages from Ciba chemicals, in Switzerland, arriving in the mail. Mischel had nothing against hippies,
but he wanted modern psychology to be rigorous and empirical. And so, in 1962, Walter Mischel moved to Palo
Alto and went to work at Stanford.
There is something deeply contradictory about Walter Mischel–a psychologist who spent decades critiquing the
validity of personality tests–inventing the marshmallow task, a simple test with impressive predictive power.
Mischel, however, insists there is no contradiction. "I've always believed there are consistencies in a person that
can be looked at," he says. "We just have to look in the right way." One of Mischel's classic studies documented the
aggressive behavior of children in a variety of situations at a summer camp in New Hampshire. Most
psychologists assumed that aggression was a stable trait, but Mischel found that children's responses depended
on the details of the interaction. The same child might consistently lash out when teased by a peer, but readily
submit to adult punishment. Another might react badly to a warning from a counsellor, but play well with his
bunkmates. Aggression was best assessed in terms of what Mischel called "if-then patterns." If a certain child was
teased by a peer, then he would be aggressive.
One of Mischel's favorite metaphors for this model of personality, known as interactionism, concerns a car making
a screeching noise. How does a mechanic solve the problem? He begins by trying to identify the specific
conditions that trigger the noise. Is there a screech when the car is accelerating, or when it's shifting gears, or
turning at slow speeds? Unless the mechanic can give the screech a context, he'll never find the broken part.
Mischel wanted psychologists to think like mechanics, and look at people's responses under particular conditions.
The challenge was devising a test that accurately simulated something relevant to the behavior being predicted.
The search for a meaningful test of personality led Mischel to revisit, in 1968, the protocol he'd used on young
children in Trinidad nearly a decade earlier. The experiment seemed especially relevant now that he had three
young daughters of his own. "Young kids are pure id," Mischel says. "They start off unable to wait for anything–
whatever they want they need. But then, as I watched my own kids, I marvelled at how they gradually learned how
to delay and how that made so many other things possible."
A few years earlier, in 1966, the Stanford psychology department had established the Bing Nursery School. The
classrooms were designed as working laboratories, with large one-way mirrors that allowed researchers to observe
the children. In February, Jennifer Winters, the assistant director of the school, showed me around the building.
While the Bing is still an active center of research–the children quickly learn to ignore the students scribbling in
notebooks–Winters isn't sure that Mischel's marshmallow task could be replicated today. "We recently tried to do a
version of it, and the kids were very excited about having food in the game room," she says. "There are so many
allergies and peculiar diets today that we don't do many things with food."
Mischel perfected his protocol by testing his daughters at the kitchen table. "When you're investigating will power
in a four-year-old, little things make a big difference," he says. "How big should the marshmallows be? What kind of
cookies work best?" After several months of patient tinkering, Mischel came up with an experimental design that
closely simulated the difficulty of delayed gratification. In the spring of 1968, he conducted the first trials of his
experiment at the Bing. "I knew we'd designed it well when a few kids wanted to quit as soon as we explained the
conditions to them," he says. "They knew this was going to be very difficult."
At the time, psychologists assumed that children's ability to wait depended on how badly they wanted the
marshmallow. But it soon became obvious that every child craved the extra treat. What, then, determined self-
control? Mischel's conclusion, based on hundreds of hours of observation, was that the crucial skill was the
"strategic allocation of attention." Instead of getting obsessed with the marshmallow–the "hot stimulus"–the
patient children distracted themselves by covering their eyes, pretending to play hide-and-seek underneath the
desk, or singing songs from "Sesame Street." Their desire wasn't defeated–it was merely forgotten. "If you're
thinking about the marshmallow and how delicious it is, then you're going to eat it," Mischel says. "The key is to
avoid thinking about it in the first place."
In adults, this skill is often referred to as metacognition, or thinking about thinking, and it's what allows people to
outsmart their shortcomings. (When Odysseus had himself tied to the ship's mast, he was using some of the skills
of metacognition: knowing he wouldn't be able to resist the Sirens' song, he made it impossible to give in.)
Mischel's large data set from various studies allowed him to see that children with a more accurate understanding
of the workings of self-control were better able to delay gratification. "What's interesting about four-year-olds is
that they're just figuring out the rules of thinking," Mischel says. "The kids who couldn't delay would often have the
rules backwards. They would think that the best way to resist the marshmallow is to stare right at it, to keep a
close eye on the goal. But that's a terrible idea. If you do that, you're going to ring the bell before I leave the room."
According to Mischel, this view of will power also helps explain why the marshmallow task is such a powerfully
predictive test. "If you can deal with hot emotions, then you can study for the S.A.T. instead of watching television,"
Mischel says. "And you can save more money for retirement. It's not just about marshmallows."
Subsequent work by Mischel and his colleagues found that these differences were observable in subjects as
young as nineteen months. Looking at how toddlers responded when briefly separated from their mothers, they
found that some immediately burst into tears, or clung to the door, but others were able to overcome their anxiety
by distracting themselves, often by playing with toys. When the scientists set the same children the marshmallow
task at the age of five, they found that the kids who had cried also struggled to resist the tempting treat.
The early appearance of the ability to delay suggests that it has a genetic origin, an example of personality at its
most predetermined. Mischel resists such an easy conclusion. "In general, trying to separate nature and nurture
makes about as much sense as trying to separate personality and situation," he says. "The two influences are
completely interrelated." For instance, when Mischel gave delay-of-gratification tasks to children from low-income
families in the Bronx, he noticed that their ability to delay was below average, at least compared with that of
children in Palo Alto. "When you grow up poor, you might not practice delay as much," he says. "And if you don't
practice then you'll never figure out how to distract yourself. You won't develop the best delay strategies, and
those strategies won't become second nature." In other words, people learn how to use their mind just as they
learn how to use a computer: through trial and error.
But Mischel has found a shortcut. When he and his colleagues taught children a simple set of mental tricks–such
as pretending that the candy is only a picture, surrounded by an imaginary frame–he dramatically improved their
self-control. The kids who hadn't been able to wait sixty seconds could now wait fifteen minutes. "All I've done is
given them some tips from their mental user manual," Mischel says. "Once you realize that will power is just a
matter of learning how to control your attention and thoughts, you can really begin to increase it."
Marc Berman, a lanky graduate student with an easy grin, speaks about his research with the infectious
enthusiasm of a freshman taking his first philosophy class. Berman works in the lab of John Jonides, a
psychologist and neuroscientist at the University of Michigan, who is in charge of the brain-scanning experiments
on the original Bing subjects. He knows that testing forty-year-olds for self-control isn't a straightforward
proposition. "We can't give these people marshmallows," Berman says. "They know they're part of a long-term
study that looks at delay of gratification, so if you give them an obvious delay task they'll do their best to resist.
You'll get a bunch of people who refuse to touch their marshmallow."
This meant that Jonides and his team had to find a way to measure will power indirectly. Operating on the premise
that the ability to delay eating the marshmallow had depended on a child's ability to banish thoughts of it, they
decided on a series of tasks that measure the ability of subjects to control the contents of working memory–the
relatively limited amount of information we're able to consciously consider at any given moment. According to
Jonides, this is how self-control "cashes out" in the real world: as an ability to direct the spotlight of attention so
that our decisions aren't determined by the wrong thoughts.
Last summer, the scientists chose fifty-five subjects, equally split between high delayers and low delayers, and
sent each one a laptop computer loaded with working-memory experiments. Two of the experiments were of
particular interest. The first is a straightforward exercise known as the "suppression task." Subjects are given four
random words, two printed in blue and two in red. After reading the words, they're told to forget the blue words and
remember the red words. Then the scientists provide a stream of "probe words" and ask the subjects whether the
probes are the words they were asked to remember. Though the task doesn't seem to involve delayed gratification,
it tests the same basic mechanism. Interestingly, the scientists found that high delayers were significantly better
at the suppression task: they were less likely to think that a word they'd been asked to forget was something they
should remember.
In the second, known as the Go/No Go task, subjects are flashed a set of faces with various expressions. At first,
they are told to press the space bar whenever they see a smile. This takes little effort, since smiling faces
automatically trigger what's known as "approach behavior." After a few minutes, however, subjects are told to
press the space bar when they see frowning faces. They are now being forced to act against an impulse. Results
show that high delayers are more successful at not pressing the button in response to a smiling face.
When I first started talking to the scientists about these tasks last summer, they were clearly worried that they
wouldn't find any behavioral differences between high and low delayers. It wasn't until early January that they had
enough data to begin their analysis (not surprisingly, it took much longer to get the laptops back from the low
delayers), but it soon became obvious that there were provocative differences between the two groups. A graph of
the data shows that as the delay time of the four-year-olds decreases, the number of mistakes made by the adults
sharply rises.
The big remaining question for the scientists is whether these behavioral differences are detectable in an fMRI
machine. Although the scanning has just begun–Jonides and his team are still working out the kinks–the
scientists sound confident. "These tasks have been studied so many times that we pretty much know where to
look and what we're going to find," Jonides says. He rattles off a short list of relevant brain regions, which his lab
has already identified as being responsible for working-memory exercises. For the most part, the regions are in the
frontal cortex–the overhang of brain behind the eyes–and include the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the anterior
prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate, and the right and left inferior frontal gyri. While these cortical folds have
long been associated with self-control, they're also essential for working memory and directed attention. According
to the scientists, that's not an accident. "These are powerful instincts telling us to reach for the marshmallow or
press the space bar," Jonides says. "The only way to defeat them is to avoid them, and that means paying
attention to something else. We call that will power, but it's got nothing to do with the will."
The behavioral and genetic aspects of the project are overseen by Yuichi Shoda, a professor of psychology at the
University of Washington, who was one of Mischel's graduate students. He's been following these "marshmallow
subjects" for more than thirty years: he knows everything about them from their academic records and their social
graces to their ability to deal with frustration and stress. The prognosis for the genetic research remains uncertain.
Although many studies have searched for the underpinnings of personality since the completion of the Human
Genome Project, in 2003, many of the relevant genes remain in question. "We're incredibly complicated creatures,"
Shoda says. "Even the simplest aspects of personality are driven by dozens and dozens of different genes." The
scientists have decided to focus on genes in the dopamine pathways, since those neurotransmitters are believed
to regulate both motivation and attention. However, even if minor coding differences influence delay ability–and
that's a likely possibility–Shoda doesn't expect to discover these differences: the sample size is simply too small.
In recent years, researchers have begun making house visits to many of the original subjects, including Carolyn
Weisz, as they try to better understand the familial contexts that shape self-control. "They turned my kitchen into a
lab," Carolyn told me. "They set up a little tent where they tested my oldest daughter on the delay task with some
cookies. I remember thinking, I really hope she can wait."
While Mischel closely follows the steady accumulation of data from the laptops and the brain scans, he's most
excited by what comes next. "I'm not interested in looking at the brain just so we can use a fancy machine," he
says. "The real question is what can we do with this fMRI data that we couldn't do before?" Mischel is applying for
an N.I.H. grant to investigate various mental …
,
Contributions of Neuroscience to Our Understanding of Cognitive Development Adele Diamond1 and Dima Amso2
1 Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, and Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
BC Children’s Hospital, Vancouver, Canada; and 2 Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychob
Collepals.com Plagiarism Free Papers
Are you looking for custom essay writing service or even dissertation writing services? Just request for our write my paper service, and we'll match you with the best essay writer in your subject! With an exceptional team of professional academic experts in a wide range of subjects, we can guarantee you an unrivaled quality of custom-written papers.
Get ZERO PLAGIARISM, HUMAN WRITTEN ESSAYS
Why Hire Collepals.com writers to do your paper?
Quality- We are experienced and have access to ample research materials.
We write plagiarism Free Content
Confidential- We never share or sell your personal information to third parties.
Support-Chat with us today! We are always waiting to answer all your questions.